Skip to main content

Delineating the autistic phenotype in children with neurofibromatosis type 1

Abstract

Background

Existing research has demonstrated elevated autistic behaviours in children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1), but the autistic phenotype and its relationship to other neurodevelopmental manifestations of NF1 remains unclear. To address this gap, we performed detailed characterisation of autistic behaviours in children with NF1 and investigated their association with other common NF1 child characteristics.

Methods

Participants were drawn from a larger cross-sectional study examining autism in children with NF1. The population analysed in this study scored above threshold on the Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition (T-score ≥ 60; 51% larger cohort) and completed the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and/or the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2). All participants underwent evaluation of their intellectual function, and behavioural data were collected via parent questionnaires.

Results

The study cohort comprised 68 children (3–15 years). Sixty-three per cent met the ADOS-2 ‘autism spectrum’ cut-off, and 34% exceeded the more stringent threshold for ‘autistic disorder’ on the ADI-R. Social communication symptoms were common and wide-ranging, while restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs) were most commonly characterised by ‘insistence on sameness’ (IS) behaviours such as circumscribed interests and difficulties with minor changes. Autistic behaviours were weakly correlated with hyperactive/impulsive attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) symptoms but not with inattentive ADHD or other behavioural characteristics. Language and verbal IQ were weakly related to social communication behaviours but not to RRBs.

Limitations

Lack of genetic validation of NF1, no clinical diagnosis of autism, and a retrospective assessment of autistic behaviours in early childhood.

Conclusions

Findings provide strong support for elevated autistic behaviours in children with NF1. While these behaviours were relatively independent of other NF1 comorbidities, the importance of taking broader child characteristics into consideration when interpreting data from autism-specific measures in this population is highlighted. Social communication deficits appear similar to those observed in idiopathic autism and are coupled with a unique RRB profile comprising prominent IS behaviours. This autistic phenotype and its relationship to common NF1 comorbidities such as anxiety and executive dysfunction will be important to examine in future research. Current findings have important implications for the early identification of autism in NF1 and clinical management.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (hereafter referred to as autism) is a neurodevelopmental disorder defined by differences in social communication and interaction, as well as the presence of restricted interests and repetitive behaviours that create limitations to activities and participation [1]. Consistent with current conceptualisation of autism as a ‘spectrum’ [2, 3], substantial variability is evident in its clinical characteristics [4]. Autistic individuals present with a wide range of cognitive and language abilities [5, 6], and commonly co-occurring difficulties such as attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and generalised anxiety disorder [7] further increase the phenotypic complexity of autism. This clinical heterogeneity is mirrored by a highly diverse genetic architecture. Large cohort studies have identified hundreds of autism susceptibility genes, comprising both common variants with low penetrance and rare variants with higher penetrance [8,9,10,11]. Despite ongoing advances in understanding how genes contribute to the development of autism, the neurobiological aetiology remains unknown in most affected individuals [12]. Moreover, the marked genetic and clinical complexity of autism has impeded the establishment of clear genotype–phenotype associations and the development of effective treatments [13, 14].

More recently, interest has turned to the investigation of syndromic forms of autism as promising candidates for elucidating the causal pathways to autism [14]. Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1) is a common monogenic syndrome, with a birth incidence of 1 in 2700 [15], which offers unique opportunities to systematically examine the autistic phenotype in the context of more homogeneous genetic aetiology. At the molecular level, mutations in the NF1 gene lead to hyperactivation of the Ras-mitogen-activated protein kinase (MAPK) pathway as well as disinhibition of mechanistic target of rapamycin (mTOR) kinase signalling [16], both of which have been implicated in the pathogenesis of autism [17, 18]. Importantly, these signalling pathways offer potential molecular targets for disease-directed pharmacological treatments in NF1 that have, so far, been elusive for idiopathic autism [19, 20]. At the clinical level, a growing body of literature has indicated an elevated autistic trait burden in NF1 [18,19,20,21,22,23,24,25,26], with recent meta-analytic evidence demonstrating a large effect size (g = 0.91) of autistic behaviours on parent-rated trait-based questionnaires [27]. Studies employing well-established diagnostic instruments have also suggested that autism prevalence in children with NF1 ranges between 11 and 26% [23, 28, 29], significantly higher than the 1–4% prevalence reported in the general population [30, 31].

As a single-gene condition with high autism penetrance, NF1 presents a valuable genetic model for advancing our understanding of the neurobiological mechanisms of autism. However, detailed characterisation of the expression of autistic behaviours in children with NF1 has been limited. Increasing our understanding of the autistic phenotype in this condition is not only important for identifying the most suitable clinical outcomes in future clinical trials, but also for the goal of establishing precise genotype–phenotype relationships. To date, three studies have attempted to quantify autistic behaviours and diagnostic rates in NF1 using the ‘gold standard’ [32] Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2) [33] and/or the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) [34]. Both the ADOS-2 observational assessment and the ADI-R parent interview comprise a range of items relating to social communication skills and restricted and repetitive behaviours (RRBs); however, some of the behaviours sampled vary across the two measures. For each measure, specified individual behaviour ratings, referred to as algorithm items, are summed into subscales and/or domain scores to provide cut-off scores for autism. Garg et al. [35] compared ADOS-2 scores of children with NF1, who scored in the clinical range on the Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition (SRS-2; T-score ≥ 60) and met cut-off for ‘autism spectrum’ on the ADOS-2, to normative data of children with autism, as reported in the ADOS-2 manual [33]. While the autistic behaviour profiles were generally similar, the NF1 group with a research-based diagnosis of autism displayed relatively better eye contact, superior language skills, and fewer RRBs than the idiopathic autism group. Another study [36] reporting on select ADI-R and ADOS-2 outcomes in children with NF1 who scored in the clinical range on the SRS-2 found mean scores for males and females on the ‘stereotyped and repetitive motor mannerisms’ ADI-R subscale to be lower relative to other ADI-R RRB subscales. However, since this study did not present the item-level data needed to understand specific autistic behaviours, the NF1 profile of RRB behaviours was not clear. More recently, Geoffray and colleagues [37] performed cross-syndrome comparisons of children who met cut-off scores on the ADOS-2 and ADI-R across NF1, Noonan syndrome, and cardiofaciocutaneous syndrome, related genetic conditions involving dysregulation of the Ras/MAPK pathway (RASopathies) [38, 39]. Subtle differences in behaviour profiles were reported between the groups, with greater social communication impairments but fewer RRBs in children with NF1 as compared to the other RASopathies. However, the evaluation of RRBs was restricted to ADI-R composite scores and the ADOS-2, which does not provide a comprehensive assessment of these behaviours [40]. This information fails to capture the full spectrum of the RRB domain as defined by DSM-5 [1], and the composite ADI-R scores potentially obscure meaningful individual differences in RRB behaviours.

Despite mounting evidence that NF1 is associated with autism [23, 28, 29], there is still active debate in the literature regarding the potentially confounding effects of the broader neurodevelopmental manifestations of NF1 on the measurement of autistic behaviours [41,42,43,44]. Some have argued that higher scores on the SRS-2 are attributable to common NF1 comorbidities of ADHD, internalising symptoms, and language problems and that such data fail to provide evidence of an increased risk for autism [41,42,43]. While the frequent co-occurrence of autism with other neurodevelopmental disorders is well established [45] and formally recognised by DSM-5 criteria [1], the careful examination of associations between autistic behaviours and other NF1 characteristics is essential for improving diagnostic accuracy in NF1. To date, these relationships have not been investigated in NF1 using gold standard autism-specific measures.

The primary aim of the current study was thus to perform detailed characterisation of the autistic phenotype in children with NF1. We conducted item- and domain-level analyses of core autistic behaviours using the ADI-R and ADOS-2 in a ‘screen positive’ NF1 cohort that scored in the clinical range on the parent-rated SRS-2 (T-score ≥ 60). This screening tool has been demonstrated to have high sensitivity (e.g. 91% for parent report) for identifying autistic children [46]. Since the ADI-R and ADOS-2 algorithm RRB items (i.e. a specified subset of items that contribute to diagnostic classification) do not capture the full range of these behaviours as defined by DSM-5, we utilised all RRB items to comprehensively assess these autism criteria. We chose a screen-positive NF1 sample for two reasons. First, existing research has demonstrated the intrinsically continuous nature of autistic behaviours in NF1 and the general population [3, 21, 26], and not limiting our sample to those who met threshold for the categorical diagnosis allowed for examination of the broader range of autistic and associated characteristics in children with NF1 who are ‘at risk’ of autism. Second, from a therapeutic perspective, children with social communication difficulties and/or RRBs may benefit from early intervention regardless of whether their difficulties fulfil formal diagnostic criteria [47]. In view of commonly co-occurring cognitive, language, and behavioural difficulties in NF1 [48, 49], and their symptom overlap with autism [50, 51], our secondary aim was to explore the associations between NF1-related neurodevelopmental comorbidities and autistic behaviours in our screen-positive NF1 cohort.

Methods

Participants

Participants were a subset of children enrolled in a larger prospective cross-sectional study investigating the autistic phenotype and social functioning in children with NF1 [52]. As part of the larger study, children were sequentially recruited from NF1 clinics at three centres: (1) The Royal Children’s Hospital/Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia; (2) The Children’s Hospital at Westmead, Sydney, Australia; and (3) the Children’s National Hospital, Washington DC, USA. All participants were diagnosed clinically with NF1 according to National Institutes of Health criteria [53]. Genetic confirmation was not available for the majority of the cohort. Participants were aged between 3 and 15 years at the time of the initial cognitive assessment. Exclusion criteria were: symptomatic intracranial pathology (other than asymptomatic gliomas), deafness, severely impaired vision, and insufficient English to complete assessments. Participants from the larger cohort were included in the current study if they scored in the clinical range on the parent-rated SRS-2 autism trait questionnaire (T-score ≥ 60), and underwent detailed evaluation for autistic behaviours using the ADOS-2 [33] and/or ADI-R [34]. Fifty-one per cent of the larger cohort were eligible for in-depth autism assessment, resulting in a final study sample of 68 children with NF1 (38 males, 30 females; see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram depicting enrolment and assessment process of participants with NF1

Direct assessments

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R)

The ADI-R [34] is a semi-structured, standardised parent interview designed to identify core autistic behaviours. It is composed of three functional domains: Qualitative Abnormalities in Reciprocal Social Interaction (Social-ADI); Qualitative Abnormalities in Communication (Communication-ADI); and Restricted, Repetitive, and Stereotyped Patterns of Behavior (RRB-ADI). Individual items concerning autistic behaviours have two ratings: ‘current’ behaviours occurring in the three months preceding the interview; and ‘lifetime’ ratings which are used to calculate the diagnostic algorithms. Lifetime ratings reflect specified behaviours at ages 4.0–5.0 years or the most abnormal behaviours to have ‘ever’ occurred in the child’s lifetime (for participants aged < 4.0 years, current/ever ratings are used). We report lifetime ratings when examining the NF1 autistic phenotype but use current ADI-R ratings when exploring associations with parent-reported psychopathology and IQ to ensure contemporaneous developmental periods are compared. Items were coded according to the examiner’s judgement of the presence or extent of a specified behaviour using an ordinal scale from 0 (absent) to 3 (present in extreme form). As per ADI-R scoring conventions, all scores of 3 were converted to 2 for the calculation of diagnostic algorithm scores. In order to meet the ADI-R classification criteria for autism, cut-offs in all three domains must be exceeded. It should be noted, however, that ADI-R cut-offs were constructed to diagnose ‘autistic disorder’ as defined by DSM-4-TR criteria [54] and are less sensitive to the broader ‘autism spectrum disorder’ presentations captured by DSM-5 diagnostic criteria [1].

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2)

The ADOS-2 [33] is a standardised, observational evaluation of the presence and severity of social communication deficits and RRBs. The current study employed either Module 2, appropriate for children capable of flexible phrase speech (n = 9), or Module 3, for verbally fluent participants (n = 56). Each item was scored on a scale from 0 to 3 with higher scores indicating greater impairment in behaviours. The Overall raw score was calculated by converting scores of 3 to 2 and summing scores from a specified subset of items. This raw score forms the ADOS-2 diagnostic algorithm, which yields an empirically derived cut-off point for ‘autism spectrum’. We also calculated 10-point calibrated severity scores (CSS) for the domains of Social Affect (SA-CSS) and RRB (RRB-CSS), as well as for the combined Overall score (Overall-CSS) [40, 55]. These comparison scores were developed within specific ADOS-2 module groupings and are less influenced by chronological age and language than raw scores [56, 57].

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale for Children-Fourth Edition (WPPSI-IV) / Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition (WISC-V)

General intellectual functioning was assessed using the WPPSI-IV [58] for children aged 3–5 years, or the WISC-V [59] for children aged 6–15 years. All subtests required for calculation of Full-Scale IQ (FSIQ) and primary index scales were administered. The FSIQ, Verbal Comprehension Index (VCI), Fluid Reasoning Index (FRI), and Visuospatial Index (VSI) were used in this study, all of which have a mean (M) of 100 and a standard deviation (SD) of 15. Higher scores reflect better performance.

Language milestones

Language milestones were elicited during the ADI-R parent interview and classified as a categorical variable (not delayed/delayed). As per the ADI-R protocol [34], Word Delay was defined as first words after 24 months of age, and Phrase Delay was defined as first 2–3 word phrases occurring after 33 months of age.

Social risk

We assessed children’s social risk via maternal education level (high risk < 12 years of schooling; low risk ≥ 12 years of schooling), as this risk factor has been found to primarily explain socioeconomic effects on child behavioural outcomes [60].

Parent rating scales

For all rating scales, sex-specific norms were used when generating standardised scores.

Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition (SRS-2)

The SRS-2 [61] is a 65-item measure designed to index the severity of autistic behaviours in the general population. We used the preschool form for children aged 3–4.5 years and the school-age form for participants older than 4.5 years. Both versions provide DSM-5 compatible subscales of Social Communication and Interaction and Restricted Interests and Repetitive Behavior [1]. Higher scores are associated with more severe symptoms, and total T-scores ≥ 60 are considered to indicate impairment in reciprocal social behaviour (M = 50, SD = 10).

Conners ADHD DSM-IV Rating Scale (CADS)/Conners-Third Edition (Conners-3)

The CADS [62] and Conners-3 [63] were used as a dimensional index of ADHD symptom severity for participants aged 3–5 and 6–15 years, respectively. Both instruments provide T-scores for Inattentive and Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales (M = 50, SD = 10). Higher scores indicate more severe ADHD symptoms.

Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL)

The CBCL/1.5–5 [64] and CBCL/6–18 [65] were administered to determine the behavioural and emotional functioning of participants aged 3–5 and 6–15 years, respectively. For both versions of the CBCL, we used the DSM-oriented subscales of anxiety problems, affective problems, and oppositional defiant problems. The former two subscales were considered ‘purer’ measures of anxiety and depression for our sample, since the CBCL broadband internalising composite may be affected by the numerous somatic manifestations of NF1 [66]. All scales were converted into T-scores (M = 50, SD = 10) with higher scores indicating more severe psychopathology.

Procedure

Ethical approval was received from the Human Research Ethics Committees of The Royal Children’s Hospital (HREC/16/RCHM/137), Sydney Children’s Hospitals Network (HREC/16/SCHN/42), and the Children’s National Hospital (Pro00007045). After explanation of the study at an initial clinical appointment, parents provided written, informed consent and completed questionnaires. Child cognitive assessments were administered by clinical neuropsychologists. If children were prescribed stimulant medication, they were requested to omit this a minimum of 24 h prior to evaluation. Participants who screened positive on the SRS-2 (T-score ≥ 60) were scheduled for ADOS-2 and ADI-R assessments, conducted by clinicians with established research reliability.

Statistical analyses

IBM SPSS Statistics (version 27) was used for all statistical analyses. Data were checked for skewness and outliers to determine whether the assumption of normality was met; where data were not normally distributed, nonparametric tests were employed. Differences between the NF1 cohort and normative reference data were tested using one-sample t tests, with Cohen’s d indicating effect size (d, small: 0.2, medium: 0.5, large: 0.8) [67]. For comparisons between NF1 inheritance type groups (familial versus sporadic), we employed independent t tests and Mann–Whitney U tests for normally and non-normally distributed continuous variables, respectively. Chi-square tests were used to examine group differences for categorical variables. The false discovery rate (FDR) procedure was used to correct for multiple comparisons [68]. We examined ADOS-2- and ADI-R-derived autistic behaviours at both item and global levels. Item-level analyses are particularly informative since current diagnostic practice focuses on determining the presence or absence of specific autistic behaviours, and global scores may mask the identification of such characteristics. Items that were endorsed by more than half the sample (> 50%) were considered ‘common’. At the global level, we reported ADOS-2 and ADI-R algorithm, domain, and subscale scores and presented the proportions of participants who met standard algorithm cut-offs for these diagnostic instruments. While ADOS-2 global scores were not normally distributed, we reported means and 95% confidence intervals (CI) to provide comparability to relevant NF1 publications [28, 35, 37].

At an individual item level, we reported the frequency of endorsed autistic behaviours (nonzero codings) in each domain with bar graphs. To provide comprehensive information regarding the severity of symptom endorsement, we also reported the frequency of 1 vs. 2/3 codings and means and SDs at an item level for the ADI-R and ADOS-2. Bivariate correlational analyses investigated associations between autistic behaviours and other developmental comorbidities in NF1 (r/rho, 0.3: weak, 0.5: moderate, 0.7: strong) [69]. Since early language delay may be considered predictive of social communication deficits, linear regressions were conducted where significant associations between language delay and autistic behaviours were found.

Results

Sample characteristics

Clinical and demographic data for the larger cohort of 152 participants with NF1 are provided as supplementary data (Additional file 1: Table S1). Characteristics for the screen-positive NF1 sample analysed in this study are shown in Table 1. FSIQ and all reported Wechsler composite scores were significantly lower (> 1 SD) than the population mean, and 7.4% of the cohort had a clinical diagnosis of intellectual disability (ID) (i.e. evidence of intellectual and adaptive impairment during the developmental period) [1]. Parent report of early language development indicated that 25% of the sample exhibited single Word Delay and 39% exhibited Phrase Delay. While all participants scored above the SRS-2 cut-off on account of our sample selection criteria, considerable variability in severity of autistic behaviours was evident, with total T-scores ranging from 60 to 99. As expected, all SRS-2 T-scores were significantly elevated relative to population norms, as were scores reflecting internalising and externalising symptoms (all, p < 0.001), with Cohen’s d indicating large effect sizes (see Table 1). There was no effect of NF1 transmission (sporadic versus inherited) on social risk (χ2 = 0.95, p = 0.33), FSIQ, and Wechsler composite scores (all, p > 0.38), and SRS-2 total T-scores (t = 0.40, p = 0.69).

Table 1 Descriptive data for the NF1 sample (N = 68)a

Core features of autism

Mean ADI-R and ADOS-2 algorithm domain scores are presented in Table 2. Approximately half the sample met ADI-R lifetime algorithm cut-offs for the Social, Communication, and RRB domains, and 34% met criteria across all three domains. The ADOS-2 ‘autism spectrum’ cut-off was met by 63% of participants, and 48% were rated as having ‘moderate’ or ‘high’ evidence of autistic behaviours, as indicated by the Overall-CSS. Over half the sample achieved comparison scores consistent with ‘moderate’ or ‘high’ evidence of autistic behaviours for the ADOS-2 domains of Social Affect (55%) and RRB (68%). Further information regarding the overlap between the proportion of participants that fell in the SA-CSS and RRB-CSS severity categories, and the contribution of SA-CSS and RRB-CSS to Overall-CSS can be found in Table S1 (Additional file 1). All ADI-R and ADOS-2 domain scores were comparable for participants with familial versus sporadic NF1 inheritance type (all, p > 0.32).

Table 2 Mean (95% CI) and proportion meeting cut-offs on ADI-R and ADOS-2 diagnostic algorithms and domains

We examined endorsement of autistic behaviours at an item level for the ADI-R lifetime ratings and the ADOS-2. Figures 2 and 3 depict the proportion of participants in which autistic behaviours were endorsed (nonzero codings) in the social communication and RRB domains, respectively. Frequency of symptom severity codings (i.e. 1 versus 2/3) with means and SDs is included as supplementary data (Additional file 1: Tables S2 and S3). For ADI-R social communication behaviours, 59% (16 out of 27 items) were rated as ‘commonly’ impaired (all > 50%, see Fig. 2a). These endorsed items spanned the areas of peer relationships, imaginative play, and reciprocal conversation, as well as use of eye contact and social smiling. Less commonly endorsed social communication impairments were use of nonverbal gestures such as nodding and head shaking, but abnormal pointing was endorsed in almost half the sample. As shown in Fig. 2b, high parent endorsement of social communication difficulties was supported by clinician ratings on the ADOS-2. Particularly pronounced were impairments in social overtures and responses as well as in reciprocal verbal and nonverbal social communication skills. Unusual eye contact and limited facial expressions were clinician-observed in close to one-half of this cohort.

Fig. 2
figure 2

a Percentage of sample endorsed with difficulties for items in ADI-R Social and Communication domains, N ranges between 61 and 65, *Friendships item (N = 30). b Percentage of sample endorsed with difficulties for items in ADOS-2 Social Affect domain (N = 65), *item specific to Module 3 (N = 56), Note: coloured bars represent DSM-5 diagnostic criteria

Fig. 3
figure 3

a Percentage of sample endorsed with restricted/repetitive behaviours on the ADI-R (N = 65). b Percentage of sample endorsed with restricted/repetitive behaviours on the ADOS-2 (N = 65), *item specific to Module 3 (N = 56), Note: coloured bars represent DSM-5 diagnostic criteria

Commonly endorsed RRBs on the ADI-R were difficulties with minor changes, undue sensitivity to noise, and circumscribed interests (all > 50%, see Fig. 3a). Infrequently endorsed behaviours included motor stereotypies, rituals, compulsions, and unusual preoccupations and attachments (all < 25%). No RRBs were coded by clinicians on the ADOS-2 in > 50% of the sample (see Fig. 3b). Compulsions or rituals and unusual sensory interests were, however, observed in 43% and 37% of participants, respectively. Consistent with parent report, there was minimal evidence of motor stereotypies on the ADOS-2 (8%).

Associations between autistic behaviours and child characteristics

Correlations between domain-level scores on the ADI-R and ADOS-2, SRS-2 total T-score, and child characteristics in our NF1 cohort are presented in Table 3. First examining relationships via the autism-specific diagnostic tools, the presence of Word Delay (r = 0.40, p < 0.001) and Phrase Delay (r = 0.43, p < 0.001) was significantly but weakly associated with higher levels of parent-reported lifetime communication impairments on the ADI-R. Univariate linear regression revealed that a positive history of Phrase Delay significantly predicted higher lifetime scores on Communication-ADI (R2 = 0.16, p = 0.001; 16% of variance explained). We only employed Phrase Delay for regression analysis as this variable showed larger associations with ADI-R scores than Word Delay, and Word Delay was highly correlated with Phrase Delay (rho = 0.615, p < 0.001). A trend was identified for a relationship between Phrase Delay and Social-ADI (r = 0.31, p = 0.012) and SA-CSS (rho = 0.32, p = 0.012). Further, we detected a trend-level association between verbal intellect (VCI) and current ADI-R Communication (r = − 0.27, p = 0.035) and ADOS-2 Social Affect domains (rho = − 0.28, p = 0.023). In contrast, RRB domains were not related to early language delay for either autism-specific measure (all; p > 0.05).

Table 3 Current ADI-R and ADOS-2 domain and SRS-2 correlations with child characteristics

Next, we examined the relationship between autistic and ADHD behaviours. We found a weak positive correlation between hyperactive/impulsive ADHD symptoms and Social-ADI (r = 0.36, p = 0.003) and trend-level associations with the ADI-R Communication (r = 0.28, p = 0.037) and RRB domain scores (r = 0.29, p = 0.018). No significant relationships were evident for any of the ADI-R domains with inattentive ADHD symptoms (all; p > 0.05). No associations were apparent between either inattentive or hyperactive/impulsive ADHD symptoms and autistic behaviours observed on the ADOS-2 (all; p > 0.05). Emotional and behavioural difficulties, as assessed with the CBCL, demonstrated negligible correlations with autistic behaviours (all; p > 0.05).

We observed a somewhat different pattern when examining associations between autistic behaviours, as indexed by the SRS-2, and child clinical characteristics. Significant weak-to-moderate correlations between SRS-2 total scores and inattentive (r = 0.42, p < 0.001) and hyperactive/impulsive ADHD symptom scores (r = 0.48, p < 0.001) were evident. We further detected significant but weak positive associations between SRS-2 scores, and Anxiety (r = 0.34, p = 0.006) and Oppositional Defiant Problems on the CBCL (r = 0.36, p = 0.003). There was a trend for a positive relationship between the SRS-2 and Affective Problems (r = 0.25, p = 0.042).

Discussion

The current study set out to characterise the autistic behaviour profile in children with NF1 using item and domain-level analyses of data obtained from the ADI-R and ADOS-2. We examined phenotypic presentation in a screen-positive NF1 sample, selected on the basis of scoring in the clinical range on the SRS-2. This provided a cohort with raised levels of autistic behaviours who may or may not meet threshold for autism diagnosis. To address the debate around whether features of autism in NF1 are driven by the broader NF1 neurodevelopmental phenotype, we also explored associations between autistic behaviours and cognitive, language, and behavioural comorbidities in our NF1 cohort. This study is the first to perform detailed global and local analyses of autistic behaviours with both gold standard measures of the ADI-R and ADOS-2, and its findings raise important points regarding the autistic phenotype in children with NF1.

First, at a global level of analysis, we found that 63% of our screen-positive cohort met the standardised cut-off for ‘autism spectrum’ on the ADOS-2 and 34% exceeded all three algorithm cut-offs on the ADI-R. It is likely that the ADI-R’s more stringent ‘threshold’ for a classification of autism contributed to the substantially smaller proportion of children exceeding all three cut-offs on this measure (including RRBs), as compared with the single cut-off provided by the ADOS-2 that does not require the establishment of RRBs. While it needs to be emphasised that meeting cut-off scores does not equate to an autism diagnosis [70, 71], these findings are consistent with prior studies that have demonstrated significantly elevated autistic behaviours in children with NF1 [23, 28, 29, 37].

Second, item-level analyses of social communication symptoms suggest a phenotype that is broadly consistent with idiopathic autism. We initially grouped individual items from these measures as per DSM-5 diagnostic criteria [1]. Items evaluating the A1 criterion of ‘Deficits in social-emotional reciprocity’ were frequently observed by clinicians and reported by parents, including quality of social overtures and responses, reciprocal social communication, and social smiling. In terms of the A2 criterion of ‘Deficits in nonverbal communicative behaviours’, abnormal use of eye contact, facial expressions, and pointing were all reported in a substantial proportion of participants. Interestingly, abnormalities in conventional gestures (e.g. clapping, nodding) were endorsed in fewer children by both clinicians and parents. Criterion A3 of ‘Deficits in developing, maintaining, and understanding relationships’ was only addressed by the ADI-R. All items applying to this criterion including interest in children, friendships, and imaginative play with peers were commonly reported as atypical, highlighting the marked peer relationship difficulties experienced by children with NF1 [26, 72]. These findings are broadly consistent with prior research [35, 37] but extend the current NF1 literature by reporting novel ADI-R item-level data. While two previous studies have reported mean scores for ADOS-2 item-level data [35, 37], these pertain to children with a research classification of autism and cannot be directly compared to our results. It is worth highlighting, however, that Garg and colleagues’ [35] conclusion of better eye contact in children with NF1 is at odds with our results that suggest similarly impaired eye contact in our screen-positive cohort to ADOS-2 normative data for children with ‘autism spectrum’ [33]. While the reason for this discrepancy is unclear, our findings are consistent with eye-tracking studies that have reported atypical eye gaze towards the face and other socially relevant information in NF1 [73, 74]. We also note differences between our sample and the cohort in Garg et al., which was diagnosed with autism based on SRS-2 and ADOS-2 cut-offs and thus exhibited at least a comparable if not more severe overall level of autistic behaviours as our screen-positive sample. Verbal IQ of the Garg et al. sample was 6 points higher than the current study’s NF1 sample; however, this difference is not a large, and we did not find evidence of an association between eye contact and VCI. As such, this between-study discrepancy is most appropriately viewed as a cohort effect and future studies are needed to help determine the weight of evidence for the prominence of atypical eye contact in children with NF1-related autism.

Third, there is evidence of a unique RRB phenotype in children with NF1. Regarding the DSM-5 B1 criterion of ‘Stereotyped or repetitive motor movements, use of objects, or speech’, motor mannerisms were infrequently endorsed on either the ADI-R or ADOS-2 (i.e. less than 23% of the sample). These findings are congruent with previous observations in children with NF1 [23, 35, 36]. Some inconsistencies were evident when examining frequency of endorsement on the stereotyped speech component of the B1 criterion across the two instruments and within the ADI-R. The ADI-R Neologisms/Idiosyncratic Language item, as defined by the use of non-words and unusual words or phrases, was rarely endorsed in our sample (11%), whereas Stereotyped Utterances/Delayed Echolalia was more commonly reported by parents (49%). Since both items may be endorsed because of a variety of behaviours, the exact nature of language abnormalities is difficult to gauge from this information. The ADOS-2 Stereotyped/Idiosyncratic Language item, endorsed in 28% of our sample, similarly incorporates a broad range of language features including echolalia, neologisms, across-person pronoun errors, repetitive utterances, and idiosyncratic or stereotyped speech. Future research is warranted to more precisely characterise the types of language abnormalities considered to constitute RRBs in children with NF1, using dedicated language measures that can better examine their relationship with structural as well as pragmatic language skills. Under the B2 criterion of ‘Excessive adherence to routines, ritualised patterns, or resistance to change’, difficulties with minor changes were commonly parent-reported (66%), with less evidence of resistance to trivial changes in the environment (22%). Interestingly, divergent patterns emerged regarding the presence of rituals and compulsions on the ADI-R (11%) and ADOS-2 (43%). Further work will be required to clarify causes of this difference, e.g. whether the interpretation of behaviours required for endorsement of compulsions and rituals varies for caregivers versus clinicians. Differential endorsement of items comprising the ‘Highly restricted, fixated interests’ B3 criterion was also observed, with common parent report of circumscribed interests (57%), but infrequent endorsement of unusual preoccupations (19%) and unusual attachment to objects (14%). Regarding the B4 criterion of atypical sensory processing, a substantial proportion of our sample were parent-reported to display sensory aversions, i.e. sensitivity to noise (60%) and abnormal responses to sensory stimuli (40%). Further, unusual sensory interests were endorsed by parents and clinicians in 46% and in 37% of our sample, respectively.

Fourth, RRBs in NF1 appear to be dominated by an insistence on sameness. Since DSM-5 categorisations of RRBs are not empirically derived [2], another way of considering these behaviours is to examine their relationship to distinct RRB subcategories identified in idiopathic autism. Factor analytic studies have supported the presence of two dimensions, ‘repetitive sensory motor’ (RSM) and ‘insistence on sameness’ (IS) behaviours. RSM behaviours comprise motor mannerisms, repetitive use of objects, and sensory-seeking behaviours, which tend to negatively correlate with age and IQ in idiopathic autism samples [75,76,77]. In turn, IS behaviours include resistance to change, compulsions and rituals, circumscribed interests, and sensitivity to noise [75,76,77,78]. In this context, it is notable that the key RRBs in our cohort were all IS behaviours, driven by difficulties with minor changes, circumscribed interests, and sensitivity to noise. This novel finding has intriguing implications for a distinct profile of autistic behaviours in NF1. We could speculate that this pattern of more pronounced IS behaviours, which have been conceptualised as ‘higher-order’ RRBs [77], may relate to the relatively higher and truncated IQ in NF1 [48], as compared to the greater prevalence of severe intellectual impairments in idiopathic autism [5]. Indeed, circumscribed interests, together with compulsions and rituals, have been found to positively correlate with IQ in autistic children [79], although some studies have shown no association or only small positive associations between IQ and IS behaviours [75,76,77]. It is also interesting to consider how common NF1 characteristics such as executive deficits [80] and anxiety [81] may affect the expression of IS behaviours in NF1, since these constructs conceptually converge and have overlapping symptom presentations (e.g. cognitive/behavioural inflexibility). Executive deficits and anxiety have been suggested to relate to IS characteristics in idiopathic autism [82, 83], but such potentially informative associations are yet to be explored in children with NF1. Moving forward, subsequent studies should investigate whether the IS and RSM dimensions found in idiopathic autism also emerge in NF1, and which specific behaviours cluster together in these, or perhaps different factors.

Fifth, our findings provided convincing evidence that the ADOS-2 and ADI-R captured distinct autistic behaviours in our cohort that were not attributable to the common NF1 comorbidities evaluated in this study. We observed weak positive associations between parent-reported hyperactive/impulsive ADHD symptoms and autistic behaviours on the ADI-R, but not on the ADOS-2. Moreover, neither ADI-R nor ADOS-2 ratings, in any domain, were related to parent-reported inattentive ADHD, internalising, and oppositional defiant symptoms. In contrast, we detected weak-to-moderate positive associations between SRS-2 scores and ratings from measures evaluating ADHD, anxiety, and oppositional defiant symptoms in our sample. These relatively stronger correlations, as compared with the ADI-R and ADOS-2, are likely due to common rater bias between the SRS-2 and the other parent rating scales, as well as reduced specificity of the SRS-2 in children with comorbid childhood neurodevelopmental disorders such as ADHD, anxiety, and high levels of challenging behaviours [50, 84, 85]. Our findings are also broadly consistent with the moderate-to-high positive associations between SRS-2 and CBCL scores reported by Morotti et al. [41], which were partly used to contend that ‘autism-like’ behaviours in their NF1 sample were better explained by ADHD and internalising symptoms. While we acknowledge concerns regarding the specificity of the SRS-2 in differentiating autism from other neurodevelopmental disorders (e.g. ADHD, anxiety) [50, 84, 85], it is important to recognise that a significant relationship between autistic and non-autistic behaviours does not ‘explain away’ autism [43]. Indeed, prior research has demonstrated substantial correlations between features of autism and ADHD in NF1 [21, 24,25,26], and it is likely that these associations reflect the tendency for these characteristics to coexist in NF1, as is the case in idiopathic autism [7].

We further detected weak-to-moderate relationships between early language proficiency and verbal intellect, and social communication difficulties on the ADI-R and ADOS-2. Again, some degree of association between these constructs would be anticipated given that language and verbal abilities have been shown to affect social communication behaviours in children with and without autism [86, 87]. It is, however, worth noting the weak and non-significant relationship between verbal intellect and current communication scores on the ADI-R in our cohort. This finding may partly be due to several items in the ADI-R Communication domain assessing nonverbal communicative behaviours that are not contingent on verbal abilities (e.g. gestures and spontaneous imitation of actions) and pragmatic language deficits impairing social communication even in the context of intact structural language and verbal skills [2, 88]. At present, the nature of these observed associations between core autism features and other clinical characteristics of NF1 are unclear, and longitudinal studies will be required to determine their causal relationships.

Our identification of a distinct autistic phenotype in children with NF1 may aid clinicians’ awareness of the presentation of autism in this population and facilitate more accurate and timely recognition of the disorder. IS behaviours such as restricted patterns of interest and ‘just right’ behaviours are relatively common features of typical early development [89] that may be less likely to be recognised as manifestations of autism than stereotyped and unusual behaviours. In this context, social communication difficulties in children with NF1, although present, may not be sufficiently pronounced in the relatively undemanding and well scaffolded home and early childcare settings to raise a flag for autism. More precise characterisation of the behaviour profile of autism in children with NF1 may also be used to help guide the design and implementation of early therapeutic interventions to ameliorate prominent and debilitating characteristics of autism in NF1. While autism-specific interventions are yet to be formally evaluated in children with NF1, positive responses to treatment have been shown to be highly variable amongst individuals with idiopathic autism [90,91,92,93]. Children with NF1-related autism likely represent a more circumscribed population in which to assess the efficacy of specific components of early behavioural intervention programs. Indeed, given evidence in the idiopathic literature that higher child cognitive and language skills and less severe autistic behaviours are associated with better treatment responses [94], we would anticipate that children with NF1 have the potential to gain significant benefits from appropriately designed interventions.

Taken together, our findings support the elevation of autistic behaviours in a significant subset of children with NF1 and underscore the importance of considering potentially confounding factors to guide interpretation of ‘autistic-like’ behaviours in NF1. Since salient features of the NF1 clinical phenotype overlap with classical autistic behaviours, scores on autism measures may be spuriously increased and not validly reflect autistic behaviours or diagnosis in the condition. Although the ADOS-2 and ADI-R are clinician-rated tools with high sensitivity and specificity for autism [32], scores on these measures have also been shown to be inflated by cognitive impairments and behavioural disorders not specific to autism [51, 95]. It is important for future research to address these issues. In particular, it is critical that research classifications of autism in NF1 are systematically compared to diagnoses ascertained through best estimate multidisciplinary clinical judgement [70, 71], taking into account the influence of non-autism-specific factors on instrument scores and diagnostic decisions. Such analyses are key to refining our use of autism diagnostic tools in NF1 and will be essential to progress our understanding of the autistic phenotype in children with NF1.

Limitations

Limitations of the current study include the following. First, the absence of genetic data in this cohort precludes examination of potentially informative associations between autistic behaviours and sequence variants within NF1, NF1 microdeletions, and the impact of common genetic variants not related to NF1. Given strong evidence for more severe phenotypic presentations in individuals with NF1 microdeletions compared to those with intragenic mutations [96], and the likely influence of common genetic variants [97], larger research studies dissecting these relationships will be an important avenue for advancing our understanding of genomic and neurobiological risk for autism in NF1. Second, our screen-positive cohort of children with NF1 represents a subset of children selected on the basis of scores in the clinical range on a screening questionnaire and thus is not representative of all children with NF1. However, given the high sensitivity of the SRS-2 for autism [46] and that many children in our sample with elevated SRS-2 scores exhibited minimal evidence of autistic behaviours on the ADOS-2 and ADI-R, it is unlikely that our screen-positive cohort failed to capture many children with significant features of autism. Third, our findings regarding the profile of autistic behaviours in NF1 were derived from a screen-positive sample, rather than children with a diagnosis of autism. This sample was chosen for investigation due to the dimensional nature of autistic behaviours. Efforts are currently underway by our research group to provide more specific conclusions regarding symptom presentation in children with a clinical diagnosis of autism. Fourth, we acknowledge that our characterisation of the autistic phenotype in NF1 was dependent on the specific behaviours indexed by the ADI-R and ADOS-2; future studies utilising different measures may identify additional autistic behaviours of relevance. Finally, although the ADI-R is constructed to minimise recall bias by eliciting concrete examples of behaviours from parents, we cannot exclude the possibility of retrospective bias affecting lifetime ratings on this measure [98]. The field would benefit from longitudinal designs to ascertain the developmental course of autistic behaviours and their relationship with other neurodevelopmental comorbidities in children with NF1.

Conclusions

In summary, this study characterises the core features of autism in children with NF1, with findings suggestive of a distinct autistic phenotype that has not been previously reported. Social communication behaviours appear to parallel those found in idiopathic autism [99]. Restricted and repetitive behaviours, however, seem to be characterised by more pronounced ‘insistence on sameness’ behaviours such as circumscribed interests, difficulties with minor changes, and sensitivity to noise, with little evidence of motor stereotypies, and unusual interests or attachments. Importantly, our examination of the relationship between autistic behaviours and common NF1 comorbidities indicates that the scores derived from gold standard autism instruments genuinely reflect features of autism in our NF1 cohort, with the caveat that these do not necessarily reflect an autism diagnosis. Our novel characterisation of the autistic phenotype in NF1 has important clinical and research implications. Early recognition of autism is clearly essential for optimising children’s long-term outcomes [100], and a clearer understanding of the autistic profile in NF1 will facilitate the development of screening tools that are more sensitive and specific to detecting autism in this population. This knowledge will also help tailor evidence-based interventions to target the more impairing features of autism in children with NF1. In addition, precise delineation of core autistic behaviours is a crucial step for the establishment of genotype–phenotype associations in NF1 that holds promise for advancing our understanding of the causal mechanisms of autism. Future studies employing larger NF1 cohorts with genetic testing will be required to investigate potential links between NF1 mutation types and the autistic phenotype in children with NF1. Subsequent research should also carefully examine similarities and differences in the autistic phenotype between children with NF1 and a clinical diagnosis of autism and children with autism from the general population with comparable cognitive abilities.

Availability of data and materials

The data set generated and analysed during the current study is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ADHD:

Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder;

ADI-R:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised

ADOS-2:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second Edition

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

CI:

Confidence interval

Communication-ADI:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Communication domain

CADS:

Conners ADHD DSM-IV Rating Scale

CSS:

Calibrated severity score

DSM:

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders

FSIQ:

Full-scale intelligence quotient

GTP:

Guanosine triphosphate

IS:

Insistence on sameness

MAPK:

Mitogen-activated protein kinase

M:

Mean

mTOR:

Mechanistic target of rapamycin

Overall-CSS:

Overall calibrated severity score

NF1:

Neurofibromatosis type 1

Ras:

Rat sarcoma

RRB:

Restricted/repetitive behaviours

RRB-ADI:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Restricted/repetitive behaviours domain

RRB-CSS:

Restricted/repetitive behaviours calibrated severity score

RSM:

Repetitive sensory motor

SA:

Social affect

SA-CSS:

Social affect calibrated severity score

SD:

Standard deviation

Social-ADI:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Social domain

SRS-2:

Social Responsiveness Scale-Second Edition

VCI:

Verbal Comprehension Index

WISC-V:

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Fifth Edition

WPPSI-IV:

Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-Fourth Edition

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®): American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.

  2. Lord C, Bishop SL. Recent advances in autism research as reflected in DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2015;11:53–70. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032814-112745.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Constantino JN, Charman T. Diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder: reconciling the syndrome, its diverse origins, and variation in expression. Lancet Neurol. 2016;15(3):279–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00151-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Grzadzinski R, Huerta M, Lord C. DSM-5 and autism spectrum disorders (ASDs): an opportunity for identifying ASD subtypes. Mol Autism. 2013;4:12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0072-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Christensen DL, Braun KVN, Baio J, Bilder D, Charles J, Constantino JN, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2012. MMWR Surveill Summ. 2018;65(13):1–23. https://doi.org/10.15585/mmwr.ss6513a1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Brignell A, Morgan AT, Woolfenden S, Klopper F, May T, Sarkozy V, et al. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the prognosis of language outcomes for individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Dev Lang Impair. 2018;3:1–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/2396941518767610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Salazar F, Baird G, Chandler S, Tseng E, O’sullivan T, Howlin P, et al. Co-occurring psychiatric disorders in preschool and elementary school-aged children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(8):2283–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2361-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Geschwind DH, State MW. Gene hunting in autism spectrum disorder: on the path to precision medicine. Lancet Neurol. 2015;14(11):1109–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1474-4422(15)00044-7.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Myers SM, Challman TD, Bernier R, Bourgeron T, Chung WK, Constantino JN, et al. Insufficient evidence for “autism-specific” genes. Am J Hum Genet. 2020;106(5):587–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2020.04.004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Yuen RK, Szatmari P, Vorstman JA. The genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Autism and the pervasive developmental disorders: Cambridge, United Kingdom: Cambridge University Press; 2019. p. 112–28.

  11. Constantino JN. Deconstructing autism: from unitary syndrome to contributory developmental endophenotypes. Int Rev Psychiatry. 2018;30(1):18–24. https://doi.org/10.1080/09540261.2018.1433133.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Patel J, Lukkes JL, Shekhar A. Overview of genetic models of autism spectrum disorders. Progress in brain research. 241: Elsevier; 2018. p. 1–36.

  13. Molosh AI, Shekhar A. Neurofibromatosis type 1 as a model system to study molecular mechanisms of autism spectrum disorder symptoms. Progress in brain research. 241: Elsevier; 2018. p. 37–62.

  14. Garg S, Green J. Studying child development in genetic models of ASD. Progress in brain research. 241: Elsevier; 2018. p. 159–92.

  15. Evans DG, Howard E, Giblin C, Clancy T, Spencer H, Huson SM, et al. Birth incidence and prevalence of tumor-prone syndromes: estimates from a UK family genetic register service. Am J Med Genet A. 2010;152A(2):327–32. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.33139.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Korf BR, Wayne H, Patterson MC, TePas E. Neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1): pathogenesis, clinical features, and diagnosis. UpToDate, Waltham, MA.: UpToDate Retrieved from www.uptodate.com; 2015.

  17. Pinto D, Delaby E, Merico D, Barbosa M, Merikangas A, Klei L, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94(5):677–94. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.03.018.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Sato A. mTOR, a potential target to treat autism spectrum disorder. CNS Neurol Disord Drug Targets. 2016;15(5):533–43. https://doi.org/10.2174/1871527315666160413120638.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Eissa N, Al-Houqani M, Sadeq A, Ojha SK, Sasse A, Sadek B. Current enlightenment about etiology and pharmacological treatment of autism spectrum disorder. Front Neurosci. 2018;12:304. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00304.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Loth E, Murphy DG, Spooren W. Defining precision medicine approaches to autism spectrum disorders: concepts and challenges. Front Psychiatry. 2016;7:188. https://doi.org/10.2174/1871527315666160413120638.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Morris SM, Acosta MT, Garg S, Green J, Huson S, Legius E, et al. Disease burden and symptom structure of autism in neurofibromatosis type 1: a study of the International NF1-ASD Consortium Team (INFACT). JAMA Psychiat. 2016;73(12):1276–84. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.2600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Adviento B, Corbin IL, Widjaja F, Desachy G, Enrique N, Rosser T, et al. Autism traits in the RASopathies. J Med Genet. 2014;51(1):10–20. https://doi.org/10.1136/jmedgenet-2013-101951.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Plasschaert E, Descheemaeker MJ, Van Eylen L, Noens I, Steyaert J, Legius E. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder symptoms in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Am J Med Genet B. 2015;168(1):72–80. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.32280.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Walsh KS, Velez JI, Kardel PG, Imas DM, Muenke M, Packer RJ, et al. Symptomatology of autism spectrum disorder in a population with neurofibromatosis type 1. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(2):131–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12038.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Garg S, Lehtonen A, Huson SM, Emsley R, Trump D, Evans DG, et al. Autism and other psychiatric comorbidity in neurofibromatosis type 1: evidence from a population-based study. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2013;55(2):139–45. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.12043.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Payne JM, Walsh KS, Pride NA, Haebich KM, Maier A, Chisholm A, et al. Social skills and autism spectrum disorder symptoms in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: evidence for clinical trial outcomes. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2020;62(7):813–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14517.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Chisholm AK, Anderson VA, Pride NA, Malarbi S, North KN, Payne JM. Social function and autism spectrum disorder in children and adults with neurofibromatosis type 1: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neuropsychol Rev. 2018;28(3):317–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11065-018-9380-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Eijk S, Mous SE, Dieleman GC, Dierckx B, Rietman AB, de Nijs PFA, et al. Autism spectrum disorder in an unselected cohort of children with neurofibromatosis type 1 (NF1). J Autism Dev Disord. 2018;48(7):2278–85. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-018-3478-0.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Garg S, Green J, Leadbitter K, Emsley R, Lehtonen A, Evans DG, et al. Neurofibromatosis type 1 and autism spectrum disorder. Pediatrics. 2013;132(6):e1642–8. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2013-1868.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. 2014;383(9920):896–910. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. May T, Brignell A, Williams K. Autism spectrum disorder prevalence in children aged 12–13 years from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Autism Res. 2020;13(5):821–7. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2286.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Falkmer T, Anderson K, Falkmer M, Horlin C. Diagnostic procedures in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic literature review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2013;22(6):329–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-013-0375-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-2nd edition (ADOS-2): Los Angeles. CA: Western Psychological Corporation; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  34. Le Couteur A, Lord C, Rutter M. The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R): Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

  35. Garg S, Plasschaert E, Descheemaeker MJ, Huson S, Borghgraef M, Vogels A, et al. Autism spectrum disorder profile in neurofibromatosis type 1. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45(6):1649–57. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-014-2321-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Garg S, Heuvelman H, Huson S, Tobin H, Green J. Sex bias in autism spectrum disorder in neurofibromatosis type 1. J Neurodev Disord. 2016;8(26):1–8. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-016-9159-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Geoffray MM, Falissard B, Green J, Kerr B, Evans DG, Huson S, et al. Autism spectrum disorder symptom profile across the RASopathies. Front Psychiatry. 2021;11: 585700. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.585700.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Aoki Y, Niihori T, Inoue S-i, Matsubara Y. Recent advances in RASopathies. J Hum Genet. 2016;61(1):33–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/jhg.2015.114.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Rauen KA. The rasopathies. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2013;14:355–69. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-genom-091212-153523.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Hus V, Gotham K, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS domain scores: separating severity of social affect and restricted and repetitive behaviors. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(10):2400–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1719-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Morotti H, Mastel S, Keller K, Barnard RA, Hall T, O'Roak BJ, et al. The association of neurofibromatosis and autism symptomatology is confounded by behavioral problems. medRxiv. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1101/2019.12.20.19015420

  42. Fombonne E, Morotti H, Mastel S, Keller K, Barnard RA, Hall T, et al. Autism questionnaire scores do not only rise because of autism. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2021;63(2):235–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14725.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Morris SM, Acosta MT, Garg S, Green J, Legius E, North K, et al. Autism in neurofibromatosis type 1: misuse of covariance to dismiss autistic trait burden. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2021;63(2):233–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Klein-Tasman BP. Are the autism symptoms in neurofibromatosis type 1 actually autism? Dev Med Child Neurol. 2021;63(2):132. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.14597.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Simonoff E, Pickles A, Charman T, Chandler S, Loucas T, Baird G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2008;47(8):921–9. https://doi.org/10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Aldridge FJ, Gibbs VM, Schmidhofer K, Williams M. Investigating the clinical usefulness of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) in a tertiary level, autism spectrum disorder specific assessment clinic. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42(2):294–300. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-011-1242-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Happe F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. 2006;9(10):1218–20. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1770.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Pride NA, North KN. The cognitive profile of NF1 children: therapeutic implications. Neurofibromatosis type 1: Springer; 2012. p. 55–69.

  49. Wang DL, Smith KB, Esparza S, Leigh FA, Muzikansky A, Park ER, et al. Emotional functioning of patients with neurofibromatosis tumor suppressor syndrome. Genet Med. 2012;14(12):977–82. https://doi.org/10.1038/gim.2012.85.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Cholemkery H, Mojica L, Rohrmann S, Gensthaler A, Freitag CM. Can autism spectrum disorders and social anxiety disorders be differentiated by the Social Responsiveness Scale in children and adolescents? J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44(5):1168–82. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-013-1979-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Grzadzinski R, Dick C, Lord C, Bishop S. Parent-reported and clinician-observed autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): implications for practice under DSM-5. Mol Autism. 2016;7(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0072-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Haebich KM, Pride NA, Walsh KS, Chisholm A, Rouel M, Maier A, et al. Understanding autism spectrum disorder and social functioning in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: protocol for a cross-sectional multimodal study. BMJ Open. 2019;9(9): e030601. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmjopen-2019-030601.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Neurofibromatosis. Conference statement. National Institutes of Health Consensus Development Conference. Arch Neurol. 1988;45(5):575–8.

  54. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders-Fourth edition-Text revision (DSM-IV-TR): American Psychiatric Pub; 2000.

  55. Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Standardizing ADOS scores for a measure of severity in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2009;39(5):693–705. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-008-0674-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Bal VH, Lord C. Replication of standardized ADOS domain scores in the Simons Simplex Collection. Autism Res. 2015;8(5):583–92. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1474.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  57. de Bildt A, Oosterling IJ, van Lang ND, Sytema S, Minderaa RB, van Engeland H, et al. Standardized ADOS scores: measuring severity of autism spectrum disorders in a Dutch sample. J Autism Dev Disord. 2011;41(3):311–9. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-1057-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Wechsler D. Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence-4th edition (WPPSI-IV). San Antonio, Tx: Pearson; 2012.

  59. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-5th edition (WISC-V): Bloomington, MN: Pearson; 2014.

  60. Bornstein MH, Hahn C-S, Suwalsky JT, Haynes OM. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development: The Hollingshead Four-Factor Index of Social Status and The Socioeconomic Index of Occupations: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers; 2003.

  61. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale, Second edition. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

  62. Conners CK. Conners’ Rating Scales-Revised. New York: Multi-Health Systems Inc.; 1997.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Conners CK. Conners. 3rd ed. Toronto: Multi-Health Systems Inc; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Child Behavior Checklist for ages 1 1/2–5: ASEBA. University of Vermont; 2000.

  65. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA school-age forms and profiles. Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth, and Families; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Gutmann DH, Ferner RE, Listernick RH, Korf BR, Wolters PL, Johnson KJ. Neurofibromatosis type 1. Nat Rev Dis Primers. 2017;3:17004. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrdp.2017.4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. 2nd ed: Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1988.

  68. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J R Stat Soc Series B Stat Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  69. Hinkle DE, Wiersma W, Jurs SG. Applied statistics for the behavioral sciences: Houghton Mifflin College Division; 2003.

  70. Vivanti G, Volkmar FR. National guideline for the assessment and diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders in Australia. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50:2252–4. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-019-03937-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Randall M, Egberts KJ, Samtani A, Scholten RJ, Hooft L, Livingstone N, et al. Diagnostic tests for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in preschool children. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2018;7:CD009044. https://doi.org/10.1002/14651858.CD009044.pub2

  72. Noll RB, Reiter-Purtill J, Moore BD, Schorry EK, Lovell AM, Vannatta K, et al. Social, emotional, and behavioral functioning of children with NF1. Am J Med Genet A. 2007;143A(19):2261–73. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.a.31923.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Lewis AK, Porter MA, Williams TA, Bzishvili S, North KN, Payne JM. Facial emotion recognition, face scan paths, and face perception in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Neuropsychology. 2017;31(4):361–70. https://doi.org/10.1037/neu0000340.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Lewis AK, Porter MA, Williams TA, Bzishvili S, North KN, Payne JM. Attention to faces in social context in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2019;61(2):174–80. https://doi.org/10.1111/dmcn.13928.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Cuccaro ML, Shao Y, Grubber J, Slifer M, Wolpert CM, Donnelly SL, et al. Factor analysis of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-R. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2003;34(1):3–17. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025321707947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Bryson S, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Mahoney W, et al. Investigating the structure of the restricted, repetitive behaviours and interests domain of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47(6):582–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01537.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Bishop SL, Hus V, Duncan A, Huerta M, Gotham K, Pickles A, et al. Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(6):1287–97. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1671-0.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  78. Gotham K, Bishop SL, Hus V, Huerta M, Lund S, Buja A, et al. Exploring the relationship between anxiety and insistence on sameness in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2013;6(1):33–41. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12(4–5):247–67. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297040600630288.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Payne JM, Hyman SL, Shores EA, North KN. Assessment of executive function and attention in children with neurofibromatosis type 1: relationships between cognitive measures and real-world behavior. Child Neuropsychol. 2011;17(4):313–29. https://doi.org/10.1080/09297049.2010.542746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Walsh KS, del Castillo A, Kennedy T, Karim AI, Semerjian C. A review of psychological, social, and behavioral functions in the RASopathies. J Pediatr Neuropsychol. 2020;6(3):131–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40817-020-00088-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Boyd BA, McBee M, Holtzclaw T, Baranek GT, Bodfish JW. Relationships among repetitive behaviors, sensory features, and executive functions in high functioning autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2009;3(4):959–66. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2009.05.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Baribeau DA, Vigod S, Pullenayegum E, Kerns CM, Mirenda P, Smith IM, et al. Co-occurring trajectories of anxiety and insistence on sameness behaviour in autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry. 2021;218(1):20–7. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.2020.127.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  84. Cholemkery H, Kitzerow J, Rohrmann S, Freitag CM. Validity of the social responsiveness scale to differentiate between autism spectrum disorders and disruptive behaviour disorders. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;23(2):81–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-013-0427-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Hus V, Bishop S, Gotham K, Huerta M, Lord C. Factors influencing scores on the social responsiveness scale. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2013;54(2):216–24. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02589.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Mody M, Belliveau JW. Speech and language impairments in autism: insights from behavior and neuroimaging. N Am J Med Sci. 2013;5(3):157–61. https://doi.org/10.7156/v5i3p157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Cochet H, Byrne RW. Communication in the second and third year of life: relationships between nonverbal social skills and language. Infant Behav Dev. 2016;44:189–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2016.07.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2014;55(5):485–94. https://doi:https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12178

  89. Evans DW, Leckman JF, Carter A, Reznick JS, Henshaw D, King RA, et al. Ritual, habit, and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev. 1997;68(1):58–68. https://doi.org/10.2307/1131925.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Vivanti G, Prior M, Williams K, Dissanayake C. Predictors of outcomes in autism early intervention: why don’t we know more? Front Pediatr. 2014;2:58. https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2014.00058.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  91. Howlin P, Magiati I, Charman T. Systematic review of early intensive behavioral interventions for children with autism. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2009;114(1):23–41. https://doi.org/10.1352/2009.114:23-41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  92. Fuentes J, Hervás A, Howlin P. ESCAP practice guidance for autism: a summary of evidence-based recommendations for diagnosis and treatment. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2021;30(6):961–84. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00787-020-01587-4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  93. Darling SJ, Goods M, Ryan NP, Chisholm AK, Haebich K, Payne JM. Behavioral intervention for social challenges in children and adolescents: a systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Pediatr. 2021;175(12):e213982. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2021.3982.

  94. Zachor DA, Ben-Itzchak E. Variables affecting outcome of early intervention in autism spectrum disorder. J Pediatr Neurol. 2017;15(3):129–33. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0037-1601444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Havdahl KA, Bal VH, Huerta M, Pickles A, Øyen A-S, Stoltenberg C, et al. Multidimensional influences on autism symptom measures: implications for use in etiological research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016;55(12):1054–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2016.09.490.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  96. Bettegowda C, Upadhayaya M, Evans DG, Kim A, Mathios D, Hanemann CO, et al. Genotype-phenotype correlations in neurofibromatosis and their potential clinical use. Neurology. 2021;97(7 Supplement 1):S91–8. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000012436.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, Mattheisen M, Walters RK, Won H, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):431–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-019-0344-8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  98. Jones RM, Risi S, Wexler D, Anderson D, Corsello C, Pickles A, et al. How interview questions are placed in time influences caregiver description of social communication symptoms on the ADI-R. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(5):577–85. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12325.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Lord C, Elsabbagh M, Baird G, Veenstra-Vanderweele J. Autism spectrum disorder. Lancet. 2018;392(10146):508–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(18)31129-2.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  100. Zwaigenbaum L, Bauman ML, Choueiri R, Kasari C, Carter A, Granpeesheh D, et al. Early intervention for children with autism spectrum disorder under 3 years of age: recommendations for practice and research. Pediatrics. 2015;136(Supplement 1):S60–81. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2014-3667E.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to sincerely thank the children and families who took part in this study and Ian Fuelscher and Stephen Hearps for their statistical advice.

Funding

This work was supported by a US Department of Defense Investigator Initiated Research Award, Neurofibromatosis Research Program (W81XWH-15-1-0619), a National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC) scholarship awarded to author AKC, a NHMRC Senior Practitioner Fellowship awarded to VAA, a Murdoch Children’s Research Institute Clinician Scientist Fellowship awarded to JMP, and the Victorian Government Operational Infrastructure Scheme.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Authors AKC, NAP, KSW, GD, KW, VAA, KNN, and JMP participated in study conception and design. Data collection was carried out by AKC, FL, KMH, NAP, KSW, AM, HD, and JMP. AKC analysed the data, and KMH, NAP, FL, AU, AB, TM, VAA, KW, and JMP assisted with interpretation. AKC drafted the manuscript, and JMP and VAA provided critical revisions. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan M. Payne.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures were approved by the Human Research Ethics Committees of The Royal Children’s Hospital (HREC/16/RCHM/137), Sydney Children’s Hospitals Network (HREC/16/SCHN/42), and the Children’s National Hospital (Pro00007045). Written informed consent was obtained from parents/caregivers of all participants.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Table S1. Descriptive data for the larger NF1 cohort and by SRS-2 cut-off (N = 152). Table S2. Number and percent of sample rated with ADOS-2 SA-CSS, RRB-CSS, and Overall-CSS severity levels. Table S3. Percent endorsement 1 vs. 2/3 codings and mean (SD) of ADI-R lifetime items and subscales. Table S4. Percent endorsement 1 vs. 2/3 codings and mean (SD) of ADOS-2 items.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chisholm, A.K., Haebich, K.M., Pride, N.A. et al. Delineating the autistic phenotype in children with neurofibromatosis type 1. Molecular Autism 13, 3 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00481-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00481-3

Keywords