Skip to main content

Gray matter covariations in autism: out-of-sample replication using the ENIGMA autism cohort

Abstract

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (henceforth autism) is a complex neurodevelopmental condition associated with differences in gray matter (GM) volume covariations, as reported in our previous study of the Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP) data. To make progress on the identification of potential neural markers and to validate the robustness of our previous findings, we aimed to replicate our results using data from the Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis (ENIGMA) autism working group.

Methods

We studied 781 autistic and 927 non-autistic individuals (6–30 years, IQ ≥ 50), across 37 sites. Voxel-based morphometry was used to quantify GM volume as before. Subsequently, we used spatial maps of the two autism-related independent components (ICs) previously identified in the LEAP sample as templates for regression analyses to separately estimate the ENIGMA-participant loadings to each of these two ICs. Between-group differences in participants’ loadings on each component were examined, and we additionally investigated the relation between participant loadings and autistic behaviors within the autism group.

Results

The two components of interest, previously identified in the LEAP dataset, showed significant between-group differences upon regressions into the ENIGMA cohort. The associated brain patterns were consistent with those found in the initial identification study. The first IC was primarily associated with increased volumes of bilateral insula, inferior frontal gyrus, orbitofrontal cortex, and caudate in the autism group relative to the control group (β = 0.129, p = 0.013). The second IC was related to increased volumes of the bilateral amygdala, hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus in the autism group relative to non-autistic individuals (β = 0.116, p = 0.024). However, when accounting for the site-by-group interaction effect, no significant main effect of the group can be identified (p > 0.590). We did not find significant univariate association between the brain measures and behavior in autism (p > 0.085).

Limitations

The distributions of age, IQ, and sex between LEAP and ENIGMA are statistically different from each other. Owing to limited access to the behavioral data of the autism group, we were unable to further our understanding of the neural basis of behavioral dimensions of the sample.

Conclusions

The current study is unable to fully replicate the autism-related brain patterns from LEAP in the ENIGMA cohort. The diverse group effects across ENIGMA sites demonstrate the challenges of generalizing the average findings of the GM covariation patterns to a large-scale cohort integrated retrospectively from multiple studies. Further analyses need to be conducted to gain additional insights into the generalizability of these two GM covariation patterns.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (autism) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterized by social-communicative difficulties, accompanied by restrictive and repetitive behaviors, and altered sensory processing [2]. The lifelong impact of autism prompts research into the etiology underpinning this condition. In the exploration of the brain substrates of autism, many anatomical magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) studies have reported that autism is associated with differences in gray matter (GM) morphology (e.g., [28]). However, divergent findings of GM morphometry have been demonstrated in multiple brain regions (e.g., [6, 11]) complicating the identification of neural correlates of autism. For example, former large-scale studies found either smaller or no changed volume with respect to the subcortical areas (e.g., pallidum) in autism [28, 31]. Furthermore, previous research reported increased or decreased volume in several cortical areas (e.g., right inferior temporal gyrus) [6, 11].

We inferred previously that divergent findings can partly be attributed to the appliance of mass univariate approaches in these studies while not considering the structural GM covariations in the brain [22]. Our previous work introduced independent component analysis (ICA) on voxel-based morphometry (VBM) data in a deeply phenotyped large autism sample, the EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP) [8, 20]. There we demonstrated that there are two autism-related brain GM covariation patterns involving (1) insula, frontal areas, and caudate, and (2) amygdala, hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG). These findings corroborated our inferences that autism is associated with the combined influence of several GM regions, and our subsequent study additionally supported the stability of the GM covariation patterns [21]. The ICA approach sensitized our analysis to GM covariation patterns in the data rather than regional differences like more traditional methods. This allowed us to identify small between-group effects contributing to progress in the identification of neural markers of autism. However, in addition to the data analytic strategy, the high neurobiological and phenotypic heterogeneity of autism produces obstacles to the generalization of a common GM covariation pattern of autism. Accordingly, validation across cohorts is crucial to make headway in identifying robust diagnostic markers that can facilitate the understanding of autism etiology.

Consequently, to test the robustness and validity of our previous findings, in this study we aimed to replicate our previous results using a large independent dataset—the Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis (ENIGMA) autism cohorts. This collaboration was established to unify preprocessing and analytic approaches worldwide, and aggregate the genetic and neuroimaging data of autistic and control individuals of all ages [28]. Within the framework of the present study, we adhered closely to the analytical pipeline of the original study. We utilized the spatial maps of autism-related GM covariation patterns identified in the LEAP dataset [22] to extract the relevant participant loadings from individuals within the ENIGMA dataset. Thereafter we explored the case–control difference in GM covariations in ENIGMA. We hypothesized that the GM covariation patterns applied to an independent dataset would also show case–control differences—thus showing the generalizability of autism-related spatial maps of LEAP to other autism cohorts.

Methods and materials

Participants

The participants were part of the ENIGMA autism cohort (http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/ongoing/enigma-asd-working-group), which aims at assembling MRI data across a wide range of autism studies. We refer to [28] for a detailed description of this cohort. The working group implemented a data freeze in December 2020, at which point there were 1833 autistic individuals and 1838 non-autistic individuals (aged 2–64 years old) from 56 sites. All individuals with autism were included based on clinical diagnosis according to DSM-IV or DSM-5. In correspondence with the inclusion and exclusion criteria of our previous study on the LEAP sample [22], we included participants with available T1-weighted images, aged 6–30 years old, with available IQ data and an IQ ≥ 50 (n = 1978). All T1 images were checked visually and participants were excluded due to structural abnormalities (e.g., enlarged ventricles, part of brain issues missing, and distortions), or excessive head motion (n = 31). This resulted in 905 autistic and 1042 non-autistic individuals from 37 sites entering the preprocessing pipeline. Local ethical approval was acquired in each participating site.

VBM estimation

The acquisition parameters of T1-weighted images at each site can be found in Additional file 1: Table S1. We implemented the same preprocessing pipeline as previously [22] to estimate the VBM images of each participant using the CAT12 toolbox (https://dbm.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat/, accessed 25 Jan 2022) in SPM12 (Wellcome Department of Imaging Neuroscience, London, UK). All T1-weighted images were first segmented into GM, white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid, which were then affinely registered to the MNI template. We normalized all segmented GM maps to MNI space using the previously customized group DARTEL (high-dimensional, nonlinear diffeomorphic registration algorithm, [1]) template in LEAP generated on LEAP sample [22]. Finally, all images were smoothed with a 4 mm full-width half-max (FWHM) isotropic Gaussian kernel. A full quality control check was applied containing calculations of quantitative quality measures based on the CAT12 pipeline that includes mean correlation from the sample homogeneity module and weighted overall image quality ratings (IQR), and visualizations of the segmentations.

Construction of independent sources of spatial variation

In our previous study, we utilized ICA to decompose all participants’ VBM images into 100 spatially independent sources (or components, ICs) of spatial variation, two of which were found to be significantly related to autism (we refer these two ICs as L-IC1, L-IC2, Fig. 1). Aligning with the objectives of our current study, we performed linear regressions (Eqs. 2–4) to apply ICA-derived components to the ENIGMA VBM data. In essence, ICA operates on the premise of a linear regression model. Specifically, we generated the ENIGMA specific participant loadings based on knowing the decomposed ICs and the VBM maps of ENIGMA sample. To ensure the complete configuration estimated for the whole brain, we utilized the original set of 100 spatial IC patterns derived from LEAP in these linear regression analyses. The LEAP 100 ICs spatial maps were thresholded to the voxel values at |z|> 3. The participant loading in the current study refers to the individual contribution of each participant to each component. The VBM input images were voxel-wise demeaned and divided by the standard deviation across participants, and we subsequently implemented regression analyses as follows:

Fig. 1
figure 1

The spatial maps of the two components of interest (i.e., group differences) in the current and initial study. The spatial maps were thresholded at 3 <|z|< 5. E-IC independent component of ENIGMA, L-IC independent component of LEAP

In Eq. (1), we represent the VBM ICA decomposition performed in the LEAP sample, which was implemented using MELODIC-ICA [4].

$$A_{{v \times s_{L} }}^{L} \sim B_{v \times i}^{L} \times C_{{i \times s_{L} }}^{L}$$
(1)

where \(A^{L}\) is a matrix with all participants’ VBM images concatenated from LEAP, and \(B^{L}\) and \(C^{L}\) are matrices containing the estimated independent spatial maps and participants’ loadings to each IC, respectively. For completeness, v denotes the number of voxels on each VBM image, sL represents the number of participants in LEAP, and i is the number of ICs (i = 100).

To obtain a primary representation of the participant loadings in ENIGMA with respect to the spatial maps from LEAP (matrix \(B_{v \times i}^{L}\)), we used the ENIGMA VBM data (\(A^{E}\)) and Moore–Penrose pseudoinverse [3] to calculate and extracted the corresponding spatial maps (two columns from \(B_{v \times 100}^{E}\)) and participant loadings (two rows from \(\hat{C}_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E}\)) of the two ICs of interest.

$$C_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E} = ((B_{v \times 100}^{L} )^{\prime} \times B_{v \times 100}^{L} )^{ - 1} \times (B_{v \times 100}^{L} )^{\prime} \times A_{{v \times s_{E} }}^{E} ;$$
(2)
$$B_{v \times 100}^{E} = A_{{v \times s_{E} }}^{E} \times (C_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E} )^{\prime} \times \left( {C_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E} \times \left( {C_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E} } \right){\prime} } \right);$$
(3)
$$\hat{C}_{{100 \times s_{E} }}^{E} = = ((B_{v \times 100}^{E} )^{\prime} \times B_{v \times 100}^{E} )^{ - 1} \times (B_{v \times 100}^{E} )^{\prime} \times A_{{v \times s_{E} }}^{E} ;$$
(4)

Equations 2–4 exhibit the three steps of regression analyses in ENIGMA that estimate the unknown matrices using the known ones based on matrix multiplications. That is, in Eq. (2) \(C^{E}\) was estimated based on the transformation of matrix multiplication using known spatial maps of the 100 ICs from LEAP (\(B^{L}\)) and all participants’ VBM images from ENIMGA (\(A^{E}\)). Similarly, the estimated spatial maps of the two ICs of interest in ENIGMA (\(B^{E}\)) were calculated using \(C^{E}\) and \(A^{E}\). At last, the estimated \(B^{E}\) was refitted to the linear model (Eq. 4) to compute the final participant loadings of the two ICs of interest in ENIGMA (two rows from \(\hat{C}^{E}\)) that were used in the further statistical analyses. The input spatial maps (\(B^{L}\)) were thresholded at |z|> 3.

Statistical analyses

Dice similarity coefficients were used to quantify the overlap between the LEAP and generated ENIGMA spatial maps. For this, the maps were thresholded (|z|> 3) and binarized.

To validate our previous findings of autism-related independent sources of spatial variation, we used a generalized linear model (GLM) to investigate group differences of the two constructed ICs from ENIGMA while accounting for the effect of age, sex, IQ, and scanner site. Initial analysis revealed striking site effects which led us to implement ComBat [12, 14] before analysis in the GLM. The GLM after ComBat contained age, sex, IQ, and site as covariates as well. Subsequently, we performed a type-II analysis of deviance to check if site effects were still significant on the age-, sex- and IQ-corrected participant loadings after ComBat correction (see Additional file 1: Section 2). Moreover, to determine the consistency of findings across sites we incorporated a site-by-diagnosis interaction term as an additional covariate. We employed type-III analysis of deviance to evaluate the significance of the site-by-diagnosis interaction term on the two ICs of interest.

In the current dataset, the total score of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic (ADOS) [18] was available in 486 autistic individuals (Additional file 1: Table S2), and it was used to examine the univariate relationship between the brain covariations and autistic trait. A GLM was employed to test the association while controlling for age, sex, IQ, and scanner site. Since there were limited additional clinical descriptive profiles available in the ENIGMA dataset (such as limited access to or availability of information on subscales of the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [26] and the Autism Diagnostic Observational Schedule 2 [19]), we were unable to repeat the multivariate analysis between brain and behavior using canonical correlation analyses in the present study.

Results

We excluded 5 participants due to segmentation failure. Furthermore, 218 participants were found to be potentially duplicate with each other as the correlations of their VBM images were unusually large (mostly r > 0.990), and therefore, these participants were excluded as well. Next, we visually checked the images with relatively extreme outliers of the image quality ratings (greater than three standard deviations from the sample mean). Accordingly, 32 participants required inspection, among which 18 participants were excluded due to insufficient data quality. Additionally, we excluded sites with less than 10 individuals to retain the statistical power within each site after preprocessing (3 sites were excluded, resulting in the exclusion of a total of 12 individuals). Consequently, there were 770 autistic individuals (667 males and 103 females, IQ > 50) and 924 non-autistic individuals (693 males and 231 females, IQ > 70) from 34 sites in the final sample (demographic information can be found in Table 1 and Additional file 1: Table S2).

Table 1 Characteristic of participants

Group differences in the isolated two GM covariations

The two components of interest (E-IC1, E-IC2), isolated in the ENIGMA dataset, showed significant case–control differences in participant loadings (E-IC1, β = 0.129, p = 0.013; E-IC2, β = 0.116, p = 0.024). The constructed brain patterns of these two components in the ENIGMA data are shown in Fig. 1, and these brain patterns showed high similarities with the L-IC maps both thresholded at |z|> 3 and binarized (dice similarity coefficient: IC1: 0.440, IC2: 0.519). The E-IC1 was primarily associated with varied volumes of bilateral insula, inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), and mainly right caudate in the autism group relative to controls. The E-IC2 was related to altered volumes of the bilateral amygdala, hippocampus, and parahippocampal gyrus (PHG) in the autism group relative to controls. In the analysis incorporating the site-by-group effect, we excluded two sites that exclusively included autistic individuals (n = 42). The analysis revealed significant site-by-group effects for the two ICs of interest (E-IC1, G2 = 75.771, p < 0.001; E-IC2, G2 = 48.956, p = 0.021). When considering this interaction effect, the group effects no longer exhibited significance for either IC (E-IC1, G2 = 0.144, p = 0.704; E-IC2, G2 = 0.285, p = 0.593). Figure 2 shows that the group differences in each site were diverse, while the differences in the LEAP dataset remained consistent across sites. The group difference of the ICs in each site can be found in Additional file 1: Table S3. After accounting for the site-by-group effect, we were unable to identify a significant main effect of the group in the ENIGMA dataset. Consequently, the stability of the two autism-spatial patterns cannot be generalized to the ENIGMA sample. As for the application of ComBat to our data, we included diagnosis, age, sex, IQ as biological covariates in the analysis for sanity check. The results remain consistent with the original results. E-IC1 and E-IC2 were found with significant group effect (E-IC1, β = 0.127, p = 0.013; E-IC2, β = 0.119, p = 0.020). The group effects were not significant when considering the interaction effect of site and group (interaction effect, E-IC1, G2 = 74.938, p < 0.001; E-IC2, G2 = 50.839, p = 0.015; group effect, E-IC1, G2 = 0.133, p = 0.715; E-IC2, G2 = 0.233, p = 0.564).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of the age-, sex-, IQ- and site-corrected (ComBat) participant loadings of the two components of interest across whole sample and within each site of ENIGMA. The forest plots demonstrate the mean and 95% confidential interval of each group in each site. The size of the mean marker ranges according to the sample size within each site for each group. The white circles in boxplots show the mean of each group across whole sample. E-IC independent component of ENIGMA, L-IC independent component of LEAP

At last, we did not find significant univariate associations between these two brain ICs and the total ADOS score in autism (E-IC1, β = − 0.911, p = 0.196; E-IC2, β = − 0.952, p = 0.086).

Discussion

In the current study, we tested the robustness and validity of our previous findings [22] in another large independent sample of autistic and non-autistic individuals—the ENIGMA autism cohort. We estimated the case–control differences of brain covariation patterns in ENIGMA based on our previously identified autism-related GM covariation patterns in LEAP. In ENIGMA, we reproduced the GM covariation spatial configurations after thresholding included most of the regions in the insula, frontal areas, and caudate and the regions in the amygdala, hippocampus, and PHG. However, the autism-related effects of these two ICs were not significant after considering the effect of site-by-group interactions. The scanner sites in ENIGMA demonstrated diverse group differences. These two IC patterns therefore cannot be generalized in the current settings. Corresponding to the previous VBM studies, the volumes of these brain areas were found to either be decreased, increased, or unaltered in autistic individuals compared to non-autistic controls across different samples (e.g., [6, 11, 23]).

The overall non-significant results could be attributed to many factors. First, the large ENIGMA dataset is limited by large cross site differences including variation in scanner type, field strength and scan acquisition parameters. Second, site effects are not limited to scan acquisition parameters but also to variation in sample selection across sites. Different distribution of sample characteristics across sites, such as sex, cognitive level, or severity of autistic behaviors, was suggested as another potential confounder [29]. In LEAP, while still faced with a multi-site cohort, scan parameters were prospectively harmonized, sample demographics were matched across sites and identical testing protocols were utilized thereby reducing the confounding issues present in ENIGMA. Nevertheless, dual regression operates on an individual participant basis, ensuring the output is not influenced by the inter-site variation. Moreover, the statistical assessments of group differences were conducted on the normalized participant loadings, thereby disregarding any site-specific scaling differences in the analyses. Additionally, we addressed site differences by utilizing ComBat, which has been shown to demonstrate good performance in controlling for site effects [12]. The site differences in our study have been effectively considered while preserving biologically meaningful variations. Despite these efforts, it cannot be conclusively stated that the failure of replication is entirely unrelated to the site differences.

The ENIGMA sample used in the current study was selected to retain a similar demographic structure to that of the LEAP sample (i.e., with same age and IQ ranges). However, achieving full feasibility was not possible due to the absence of individuals in the control group of ENIGMA with an IQ below 70. Additionally, attempting to maintain a representative sample of autistic individuals, we avoided matching ENIGMA sample to LEAP. Consequently, there are notable differences in the descriptive distributions between the two samples (please see Additional file 1: Table S4). Moreover, in ENIGMA, limited information of the co-occurring psychiatric conditions (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, anxiety) and medication use could impact the outcomes.

When site effects were effectively removed, the results between diagnostic groups significantly differed across sites in the current study. The discrepancy of results varies within sites may relate to the high clinical, biological, and etiologic heterogeneity within autistic individuals [17]. For our analysis, categorical groups were defined according to clinical diagnosis, which depended on the observations of the behaviors under a definition of autism in the developing diagnostic criteria (e.g., DSM-IV to DSM-5). The changing criteria unavoidably increased the heterogeneity within those with an autism diagnosis, at least at the level of behavioral manifestations, and likely also within research populations—especially those collected over longer periods. For instance, the DSM-5 criteria consolidated autism as a spectrum with excluding the language impairments and including altered sensory responses as a defined group of traits [2]. Additionally, biological phenotypes (endpoints), such as GM volume and behavioral phenotypes, can be traced back to diverse mechanisms (i.e., equifinality in the developmental psychopathology) [10, 27]. Moreover, the genetic contributions to autism are extremely complex. These include inherited and/or de novo genetic mutations, de novo or inherited copy number variations, common genetic variants, and associations with syndromal forms of autism [9, 25]. In addition, there are multiple other suggested factors, such as advanced maternal and paternal age and prenatal exposure to sex steroids, or medications like valproate [5]. The etiological and behavioral heterogeneity of autism increases the difficulty of identifying a general GM pattern for autism and of replicating them as average group effects may be hidden by individual variability varying between sites.

Our findings here reproduced the GM covariation spatial configurations that we found to be related to autism in LEAP. These covaried brain areas have been reported relating to autism widely in previous studies and have primarily been implicated in autism-related social and non-social behaviors [7, 13, 15, 16, 24]. The importance of the covariations of insula, IFG and OFC was emphasized in our multimodal study [21]. The gray matter covariations patterns of these regions shared variance with diffusion weighted imaging metrics and related to autism as well. In our previous study, we used a canonical correlation analysis to address within group variability and relate GM covariation patterns to various behavioral measures. Unfortunately, this was not an option with the ENIGMA data as only limited continuous measures of behavior were available.

On the one hand, big datasets, like ENIGMA, are exploited as the increased sample size improves the possibility of identifying effects with small effect size. However on the other hand, the retrospective pooling of data from multiple studies can introduce more heterogeneity than the increase in power can offset. While heterogeneity may seem like a hindrance to research it is not and it is important to involve a heterogenous ‘representative’ autistic sample with not only large sample size but multiple levels of data [17]. Utilizing multivariate/multimodal approaches in future studies and focusing on subtyping or continuous behavioral measures, rather than categorical group differences, may be more promising avenues for biomarker discovery and validation. Additionally, future studies dedicated to individual differences in autism probing heterogeneity could provide more insight for biomarker discovery, for example, normative modeling, which has been applied to the LEAP cohort [30]. The effect of group differences in cortical measure may vary across different subgroups.

Our findings should be interpreted at the background of several limitations. As we selected only participants with available IQ estimates, this probably induced some sample selection bias. Sex distribution, and IQ did not match across autistic and non-autistic groups, however we controlled for the effects of these two factors in the analyses. Moreover, the distributions of age, IQ and sex between LEAP and ENIGMA are statistically different from each other. Therefore, more validation analyses in the future will be done in the IQ and sex matched subsamples. Additionally, small samples and/or autism-only samples at multiple sites could potentially influence the outcome. Owing to limited access to the behavioral data of the autism group, we were unable to further our understanding of the dimensional behaviors of the sample.

In summary, the current study is unable to replicate the autism-related brain patterns from LEAP to ENIGMA cohorts. The diverse group effects across sites of ENIGMA demonstrate the challenges of generalizing the average findings of the GM covariation patterns to another large-scale cohort integrated retrospectively from multiple studies. Subsequent studies could utilize a sample with more comparable demographic information or prioritize subtyping to gain additional insights into the generalizability of these two GM covariation patterns.

Availability of data and materials

This study utilized 34 distinct datasets collected globally, each to diverse consent procedures and regulatory authorities. Access requests to these datasets will be evaluated in accordance with the corresponding consent agreements, guidelines, and regulations. You can submit requests through the ENIGMA consortium's ASD working group at http://enigma.ini.usc.edu/ongoing/enigma-asd-working-group/.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule–Generic

ENIGMA:

Enhancing Neuroimaging Genetics Through Meta-Analysis

FWHM:

Full-width half-max

GLM:

Generalized linear model

GM:

Gray matter

IC:

Independent component

ICA:

Independent component analysis

IFG:

Inferior frontal gyrus

IQR:

Image quality ratings

LEAP:

Longitudinal European Autism Project

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

OFC:

Orbitofrontal cortex

PHG:

Parahippocampal gyrus

VBM:

Voxel-based morphometry

References

  1. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. 2007;38:95–113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Author: Arlington; 2013.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  3. Barata JCA, Hussein MS. The Moore–Penrose pseudoinverse: a tutorial review of the theory. Braz J Phys. 2012;42:146–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Beckmann CF, Smith SM. Probabilistic independent component analysis for functional magnetic resonance imaging. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. 2004;23:137–52.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Bolte S, Girdler S, Marschik PB. The contribution of environmental exposure to the etiology of autism spectrum disorder. Cell Mol Life Sci. 2019;76:1275–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Carlisi CO, Norman LJ, Lukito SS, Radua J, Mataix-Cols D, Rubia K. Comparative multimodal meta-analysis of structural and functional brain abnormalities in autism spectrum disorder and obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;82:83–102.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Chan MMY, Han YMY. Differential mirror neuron system (MNS) activation during action observation with and without social-emotional components in autism: a meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies. Mol Autism. 2020;11:72.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Charman T, Loth E, Tillmann J, Crawley D, Wooldridge C, Goyard D, Ahmad J, Auyeung B, Ambrosino S, Banaschewski T, Baron-Cohen S, Baumeister S, Beckmann C, Bolte S, Bourgeron T, Bours C, Brammer M, Brandeis D, Brogna C, de Bruijn Y, Chakrabarti B, Cornelissen I, Acqua FD, Dumas G, Durston S, Ecker C, Faulkner J, Frouin V, Garces P, Ham L, Hayward H, Hipp J, Holt RJ, Isaksson J, Johnson MH, Jones EJH, Kundu P, Lai MC, D’Ardhuy XL, Lombardo MV, Lythgoe DJ, Mandl R, Mason L, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Moessnang C, Mueller N, O’Dwyer L, Oldehinkel M, Oranje B, Pandina G, Persico AM, Ruggeri B, Ruigrok ANV, Sabet J, Sacco R, Caceres ASJ, Simonoff E, Toro R, Tost H, Waldman J, Williams SCR, Zwiers MP, Spooren W, Murphy DGM, Buitelaar JK. The EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP): clinical characterisation. Mol Autism. 2017;8:27.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Choi L, An JY. Genetic architecture of autism spectrum disorder: lessons from large-scale genomic studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;128:244–57.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Cicchetti D, Toth SL. The past achievements and future promises of developmental psychopathology: the coming of age of a discipline. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:16–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. DeRamus TP, Kana RK. Anatomical likelihood estimation meta-analysis of grey and white matter anomalies in autism spectrum disorders. Neuroimage Clin. 2015;7:525–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Fortin JP, Cullen N, Sheline YI, Taylor WD, Aselcioglu I, Cook PA, Adams P, Cooper C, Fava M, McGrath PJ, McInnis M, Phillips ML, Trivedi MH, Weissman MM, Shinohara RT. Harmonization of cortical thickness measurements across scanners and sites. Neuroimage. 2018;167:104–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Habata K, Cheong Y, Kamiya T, Shiotsu D, Omori IM, Okazawa H, Jung M, Kosaka H. Relationship between sensory characteristics and cortical thickness/volume in autism spectrum disorders. Transl Psychiatry. 2021;11:616.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Johnson WE, Li C, Rabinovic A. Adjusting batch effects in microarray expression data using empirical Bayes methods. Biostatistics. 2007;8:118–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Klapwijk ET, Aghajani M, Colins OF, Marijnissen GM, Popma A, van Lang ND, van der Wee NJ, Vermeiren RR. Different brain responses during empathy in autism spectrum disorders versus conduct disorder and callous-unemotional traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57:737–47.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Langen M, Bos D, Noordermeer SD, Nederveen H, van Engeland H, Durston S. Changes in the development of striatum are involved in repetitive behavior in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76:405–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Lombardo MV, Lai MC, Baron-Cohen S. Big data approaches to decomposing heterogeneity across the autism spectrum. Mol Psychiatry. 2019;24:1435–50.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:205–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnosticobservation schedule, second edition (ADOS-2) manual (part I): modules 1–4. Torrance: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Loth E, Charman T, Mason L, Tillmann J, Jones EJH, Wooldridge C, Ahmad J, Auyeung B, Brogna C, Ambrosino S, Banaschewski T, Baron-Cohen S, Baumeister S, Beckmann C, Brammer M, Brandeis D, Bolte S, Bourgeron T, Bours C, de Bruijn Y, Chakrabarti B, Crawley D, Cornelissen I, Acqua FD, Dumas G, Durston S, Ecker C, Faulkner J, Frouin V, Garces P, Goyard D, Hayward H, Ham LM, Hipp J, Holt RJ, Johnson MH, Isaksson J, Kundu P, Lai MC, D’Ardhuy XL, Lombardo MV, Lythgoe DJ, Mandl R, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Moessnang C, Mueller N, O’Dwyer L, Oldehinkel M, Oranje B, Pandina G, Persico AM, Ruigrok ANV, Ruggeri B, Sabet J, Sacco R, Caceres ASJ, Simonoff E, Toro R, Tost H, Waldman J, Williams SCR, Zwiers MP, Spooren W, Murphy DGM, Buitelaar JK. The EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project (LEAP): design and methodologies to identify and validate stratification biomarkers for autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2017;8:24.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Mei T, Forde NJ, Floris DL, Dell’Acqua F, Stones R, Ilioska I, Durston S, Moessnang C, Banaschewski T, Holt RJ, Baron-Cohen S, Rausch A, Loth E, Oakley B, Charman T, Ecker C, Murphy DGM, group, Eu-Aims Leap, Beckmann CF, Llera A, Buitelaar JK. Autism is associated with interindividual variations of gray and white matter morphology. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2022;8:1084–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Mei T, Llera A, Floris DL, Forde NJ, Tillmann J, Durston S, Moessnang C, Banaschewski T, Holt RJ, Baron-Cohen S, Rausch A, Loth E, Dell’Acqua F, Charman T, Murphy DGM, Ecker C, Beckmann CF, Buitelaar JK, group, Eu-Aims Leap. Gray matter covariations and core symptoms of autism: the EU-AIMS Longitudinal European Autism Project. Mol Autism. 2020;11:86.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Pappaianni E, Siugzdaite R, Vettori S, Venuti P, Job R, Grecucci A. Three shades of grey: detecting brain abnormalities in children with autism using source-, voxel- and surface-based morphometry. Eur J Neurosci. 2018;47:690–700.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Patriquin MA, DeRamus T, Libero LE, Laird A, Kana RK. Neuroanatomical and neurofunctional markers of social cognition in autism spectrum disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2016;37:3957–78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Richards C, Jones C, Groves L, Moss J, Oliver C. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder phenomenology in genetic disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. 2015;2:909–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism diagnostic interview-revised. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  27. Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Samocha KE, Cicek AE, Murtha MT, Bal VH, Bishop SL, Dong S, Goldberg AP, Jinlu C, Keaney JF 3rd, Klei L, Mandell JD, Moreno-De-Luca D, Poultney CS, Robinson EB, Smith L, Solli-Nowlan T, Su MY, Teran NA, Walker MF, Werling DM, Beaudet AL, Cantor RM, Fombonne E, Geschwind DH, Grice DE, Lord C, Lowe JK, Mane SM, Martin DM, Morrow EM, Talkowski ME, Sutcliffe JS, Walsh CA, Yu TW, Autism Sequencing, Consortium, Ledbetter DH, Martin CL, Cook EH, Buxbaum JD, Daly MJ, Devlin B, Roeder K, State MW. Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 risk loci. Neuron. 2015;87:1215–33.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. van Rooij D, Anagnostou E, Arango C, Auzias G, Behrmann M, Busatto GF, Calderoni S, Daly E, Deruelle C, Di Martino A, Dinstein I, Duran FLS, Durston S, Ecker C, Fair D, Fedor J, Fitzgerald J, Freitag CM, Gallagher L, Gori I, Haar S, Hoekstra L, Jahanshad N, Jalbrzikowski M, Janssen J, Lerch J, Luna B, Martinho MM, McGrath J, Muratori F, Murphy CM, Murphy DGM, O’Hearn K, Oranje B, Parellada M, Retico A, Rosa P, Rubia K, Shook D, Taylor M, Thompson PM, Tosetti M, Wallace GL, Zhou F, Buitelaar JK. Cortical and subcortical brain morphometry differences between patients with autism spectrum disorder and healthy individuals across the lifespan: results from the ENIGMA ASD working group. Am J Psychiatry. 2018;175:359–69.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Yamashita A, Yahata N, Itahashi T, Lisi G, Yamada T, Ichikawa N, Takamura M, Yoshihara Y, Kunimatsu A, Okada N, Yamagata H, Matsuo K, Hashimoto R, Okada G, Sakai Y, Morimoto J, Narumoto J, Shimada Y, Kasai K, Kato N, Takahashi H, Okamoto Y, Tanaka SC, Kawato M, Yamashita O, Imamizu H. Harmonization of resting-state functional MRI data across multiple imaging sites via the separation of site differences into sampling bias and measurement bias. PLoS Biol. 2019;17: e3000042.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Zabihi M, Oldehinkel M, Wolfers T, Frouin V, Goyard D, Loth E, Charman T, Tillmann J, Banaschewski T, Dumas G, Holt R, Baron-Cohen S, Durston S, Bolte S, Murphy D, Ecker C, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF, Marquand AF. Dissecting the heterogeneous cortical anatomy of autism spectrum disorder using normative models. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4:567–78.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Zhang W, Groen W, Mennes M, Greven C, Buitelaar J, Rommelse N. Revisiting subcortical brain volume correlates of autism in the ABIDE dataset: effects of age and sex. Psychol Med. 2018;48:654–68.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all participants and their families for participating in this study. We gratefully acknowledge the contributions of all members of the ENIGMA autism working group: Daan van Rooij, Evdokia Anagnostou, Celso Arango, Guillaume Auzias, Marlene Behrmann, Geraldo F. Busatto, Sara Calderoni, Eileen Daly, Christine Deruelle, Adriana Di Martino, Ilan Dinstein, Fabio Luis Souza Duran, Sarah Durston, Christine Ecker, Damien Fair, Jennifer Fedor, Jackie Fitzgerald, Christine M. Freitag, Louise Gallagher, Ilaria Gori, Shlomi Haar, Liesbeth Hoekstra, Neda Jahanshad, Maria Jalbrzikowski, Joost Janssen, Jason Lerch, Beatriz Luna, Mauricio Moller Martinho, Jane McGrath, Filippo Muratori, Clodagh M. Murphy, Declan G. M. Murphy, Kirsten O’Hearn, Bob Oranje, Mara Parellada, Alessandra Retico, Pedro Rosa, Katya Rubia, Devon Shook, Margot Taylor, Paul M. Thompson, Michela Tosetti, Gregory L. Wallace, Fengfeng Zhou, Jan K. Buitelaar.

Funding

ENIGMA received funding from NIH Consortium Grant U54 EB020403 to Paul Thompson, supported by a cross-NIH alliance that funds Big Data to Knowledge Centers of Excellence (BD2K). This research was further supported by the European Community’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) under grant agreement number 278948 (TACTICS), and the Innovative Medicines Initiative Joint Undertaking under grant agreement number 115300 (EU-AIMS), resources of which are composed of financial contributions from the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) and the European Federation of Pharmaceutical Industries and Associations companies’ in-kind contribution. TM is supported by a China Scholarship Council grant (No 201806010408). DLF is supported by funding from the European Union’s Horizon 2020 research and innovation program under the Marie Skłodowska-Curie grant agreement No 101025785. This work has been further supported by the European Union Seventh Framework Programme Grant Nos. 602805 (AGGRESSOTYPE) (to JKB), 603016 (MATRICS) (to JKB), and 278948 (TACTICS) (to JKB); European Community’s Horizon 2020 Programme (H2020/2014–2020) Grant Nos. 643051 (MiND) (to JKB), 642996 (BRAINVIEW) (to JKB) and 847818 (CANDY) (to JKB and CFB); the Netherlands Organization for Scientific Research VICI Grant No. 2020/TTW/00836465 (to CFB); Wellcome Trust Collaborative Award Grant No. 215573/Z/19/Z (to CFB).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

DvR and JKB conceptualized and developed analysis protocols of ENIGMA ASD working group. TM ran analyses, visualized the findings, wrote and revised the draft; AL and NJF conceptualized and supervised the analysis, and revised the manuscript; DLF revised the draft; CFB and JKB supervised and revised the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ting Mei.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate and for publication

The local ethical approval to carry out the research was granted in each data site, and all contributing sites in the current study had local ethical approval to share the anonymized individual data. The informed consent for participation and publication was obtained from all individuals.

Competing interests

CFB is director and shareholder in SBGNeuro Ltd. JKB has been a consultant to, advisory board member of, and a speaker for Janssen Cilag BV, Eli Lilly, Shire, Lundbeck, Roche, and Servier. He is not an employee of any of these companies, and not a stock shareholder of any of these companies. He has no other financial or material support, including expert testimony, patents or royalties. The present work is unrelated to the above grants and relationships. The other authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Supplementary Tables and Figure.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mei, T., Llera, A., Forde, N.J. et al. Gray matter covariations in autism: out-of-sample replication using the ENIGMA autism cohort. Molecular Autism 15, 3 (2024). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-024-00583-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-024-00583-8

Keywords