Skip to main content

Using the big data approach to clarify the structure of restricted and repetitive behaviors across the most commonly used autism spectrum disorder measures

Abstract

Background

Restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB) in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) encompass several distinct domains. However, commonly used general ASD measures provide broad RRB scores rather than assessing separate RRB domains. The main objective of the current investigation was to conduct a psychometric evaluation of the ability of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2), the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) to capture different RRB constructs.

Methods

Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (ESEM) was conducted using individual item-level data from the SRS-2, SCQ, ADI-R and the ADOS. Data were obtained from five existing publicly available databases. For the SRS-2, the final sample consisted of N = 16,761 individuals (Mage = 9.43, SD = 3.73; 18.5% female); for the SCQ, of N = 15,840 (Mage = 7.99, SD = 4.06; 18.1% female); for the ADI-R, of N = 8985 (Mage = 8.86, SD = 4.68; 19.4% female); and for the ADOS, of N = 6314 (Mage = 12.29, SD = 6.79; 17.7% female).

Results

The three-factor structure provided the most optimal and interpretable fit to data for all measures (comparative fit index ≥ .983, Tucker Lewis index ≥ .966, root mean square error of approximation ≤ .028). Repetitive-motor behaviors, insistence on sameness and unusual or circumscribed interests factors emerged across all instruments. No acceptable fit was identified for the ADOS.

Limitations

The five datasets used here afforded a large as well as wide distribution of the RRB item scores. However, measures used for establishing convergent and divergent validity were only available for a portion of the sample.

Conclusions

Reported findings offer promise for capturing important RRB domains using general ASD measures and highlight the need for measurement development.

Background

Restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests (RRB) have been recognized as a core symptom domain of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) since the original clinical descriptions by Leo Kanner [1]. RRB can be a major barrier to learning and adaptation, and a source of stress and management challenges for families [2,3,4,5]. Despite clinical significance, RRB domain remains poorly understood. Inconsistencies in the organization, division and measurement have had a particularly negative impact on RRB research [4, 6, 7]. Most notably, although recent investigations provide evidence that RRB are not a unitary construct but encompass several distinct domains [8,9,10,11], the ability of commonly used diagnostic and quantitative ASD measures to assess these domains separately has not been systematically evaluated. By conflating distinct constructs, overly broad domain scores can cloud the understanding of RRB trajectories, the recognition of associations with other clinical domains and hinder understanding of neurobiological mechanisms. Given the wide use of measures such as the Social Responsiveness Scale-2 (SRS-2 [12]), the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ [13]), and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS [14]) across research and clinical contexts, and their availability in several large-scale databases that also include neuroimaging and genetic data, rigorous psychometric evaluation of the ability of these instruments to capture different RRB constructs is needed.

RRB is an umbrella term for a set of heterogeneous behaviors that include motor stereotypies, daily routines and rituals, repetitive play, preoccupations with particular interests and compulsive activities related to these preoccupations. Given this complexity and heterogeneity, RRB are best understood as a multidimensional construct with a range of distinct behavioral domains [4, 15,16,17]. A variety of approaches have been taken to organize and define RRB in ASD. Approach based on clinical and developmental considerations, initiated by Prior and Macmillan [16] and significantly expanded by Turner [17] suggested that RRB can be divided into broad ‘lower-order’ and ‘higher-order’ domains based on their pattern of emergence during normative development and the relationship with IQ/cognitive functioning. Our current understanding of RRB in ASD has been influenced by the factor analytic explorations of the currently available dedicated RRB measures including the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R [18]), the Repetitive Behavior Questionnaire (RBQ [19]), the Repetitive Behavior Questionnaire-2 (RBQ-2 [20,21,22]), the Childhood Routines Inventory (CRI [23, 24]) and the Children's Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale (CY-BOCS [25]). Initial factor analytic work suggested two factor-solution encompassing Repetitive Motor Behaviors (RMB) domain which included behaviors such as hand and finger mannerisms, stereotyped body movements, and repetitive manipulation of objects or parts of objects, and Insistence on Sameness (IS) domain which included behaviors such as ritualistic behaviors and resistance to changes in routine or personal environment [26, 27]. However, studies by Leekam and colleagues [22], Lam, Bodfish and Piven [11] and Honey et al. [15] highlighted unusual/circumscribed interests (CI) with over-focus on specific topics, objects, stimuli and/or activities as an additional domain. This three-factor structure has been largely replicated across subsequent studies (e.g., [8]).

To evaluate the validity of RMB, IS and CI, several studies have explored relationships of these three RRB domains with age, cognitive ability and co-occurring symptoms such as anxiety. For instance, RMB tend to be related to younger age and lower IQ [8, 26] and are relatively independent of anxiety levels [11]. By contrast, some, but not all studies, have observed associations between IS and CI with older age and higher IQ [5, 28, 29]. IS has been consistently associated with more severe anxiety [11, 30,31,32,33]. Preliminary evidence also suggests that RMB, IS and CI domains have distinct neural [34] and genetic underpinnings [35,36,37,38] and show different familial patterns [10, 39, 40, 41].

Despite the mounting evidence suggesting the multidimensionality of the RRB, the factor structure of this domain captured by widely used screening, diagnostic and quantitative severity ASD measures remains unexplored. The only exception is the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R [42]) which has been evaluated in several studies [8, 28, 37]. However, although RMB, IS and CI factors seem to provide a relatively stable explanation of the ADI-R RRB factor structure, the actual item content of these factors has varied considerably across investigations. For instance, depending on the study, unusual attachment to objects and unusual preoccupations items have loaded onto RMB [8, 28, 37], IS [43] and CI [10, 15] factors or were not included in the final factor solution [26, 30].

Although diagnostic and quantitative ASD measures such as the SRS-2, the SCQ, the ADI-R and the ADOS have not been specifically designed to provide an in-depth, fine-grained assessment of distinct RRB domains such as RMB, IS and CI, these instruments are widely used across ASD research and clinical practice and have been included in several large datasets that are available to the research community (e.g., Simons Simplex Collection, Autism Genetic Resource Exchange, Autism Brain Imaging Data Exchange). Establishing the ability of noted measures to capture distinct RRB domains, even in a manner that is somewhat suboptimal when compared to dedicated instruments such as the RBS-R, the RBQ-2, the CRI or the CY-BOCS, can provide a valuable resource for RRB-related investigations across both clinical and research contexts. Therefore, the present study aimed to capitalize on the availability of large, publicly available databases to conduct a stringent evaluation of the RRB factor structure across the SRS-2, the SCQ, the ADI-R and the ADOS. We predicted that RMB, IS and CI factors would emerge across the majority of measures, with a potential exception of the ADOS due to the limited range of items. Given the focus of measures developed in the context of ASD and utilization of existing sample, derived findings specifically speak to the RRB structure in ASD. Future studies utilizing samples that span normative and atypical development and measures sampling RRB symptoms and domains that occur across a range of clinical populations and typical early development are needed to derive more general, transdiagnostic RRB structure. In addition to investigating RRB factor structure across the SRS-2, SCQ, ADI-R and ADOS, an important further step was to appraise the validity of the derived factors by exploring their convergence with a well-established, dedicated RRB measure—the RBS-R and the pattern of relationships with external validators such as age, sex, cognitive functioning, anxiety and disruptive behaviors. Crucially, despite the potential short-term utility of the factors derived here, the longer-term goal of the field should be to utilize quantitative instruments specifically developed to comprehensively capture distinct RRB domains. Therefore, an additional aim of this analysis was to derive information necessary for future measurement development.

Methods

Research participants

Data were obtained from five publicly available databases: Healthy Brain Network (HBN [44]); National Database for Autism Research (NDAR; https://ndar.nih.gov); Simons Simplex Collection (SSC [45]); Autism Genetic Research Exchange (AGRE [46]); and Interactive Autism Research Database (IAN; http://iancommunity.org). Only individuals with item-level data and with a diagnosis of ASD were included in this study. All participants or their parent/legal guardians have provided informed consent for participation as part of the original investigations. For the SRS-2, the final sample consisted of N = 16,761 individuals (Mage = 9.43, SD = 3.73; 18.5% female); for the SCQ, N = 15,840 (Mage = 7.99, SD = 4.06; 18.1% female); for the ADI-R, N = 8985 (Mage = 8.86, SD = 4.68; 19.4% female); and for the ADOS modules 3 and 4, of N = 6314 (Mage = 12.29, SD = 6.79; 17.7% female). Table 1 presents demographic characteristics for each of the measures broken down by each database.

Table 1 Demographic characteristics of youth participants in publicly available data sets

Measures

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS [12]) is a 65-item measure designed to index severity in social impairments and the presence of RRB. Each item is rated on a 4-point scale (from 1 = Not True to 4 = Almost Always True) with higher scores indicating higher trait severity/atypicality.

The Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ [13]) is a 40-item parent-report questionnaire designed to index the severity of impairments in social, communication and RRB domains seen in ASD. Each item is scored using the dichotomous response format, with a value of one indicating the presence of atypicality and a value of zero the absence of atypicality. In this investigation, we have focused on the lifetime ratings.

The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R [42]) is a semi-structured parent interview informed by the ICD-10 and DSM-IV intended to assist with the diagnosis of ASD. In this investigation, we have primarily focused on the current scores; however, supplemental analysis of the ever scores was also conducted and is included in Additional file 1: Table S1.

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS [14]) is a semi-structured instrument that allows direct observation of children during specific play, social and language tasks. The ADOS consists of four modules based on the chronological age and level of expressive language. Modules 1 and 2 were not considered in this analysis given that one of the items (D4) at the same time assesses IS, CI and repetitive manipulation of objects. Further, Modules 3 and 4 have an additional item that captures routines and ritualistic behaviors (item D5). Therefore, while not ideal, Modules 3 and 4 present with higher opportunity to derive distinct, fine-grained factors.

The Repetitive Behaviors Scale-Revised (RBS-R [18]) is a 43 item parent-report measure designed to capture a wide range of RRB. Although it was originally proposed that the RBS-R items constitute six distinct subscales, subsequent factor analyses mostly failed to support the original 6-factor solution. A five-factor solution derived by Bishop and colleagues [8] was utilized here with the Stereotypy factor corresponding to RMB, the Ritualistic/Sameness factor corresponding to IS and the Restricted Interests factor corresponding to CI.

For each of the measures, items were selected based on the manual. In addition, given the sometimes unclear boundaries between RRB and social communication symptoms (e.g., talking excessively about topics of special interest), the first and last author reviewed individual items across the measures to ensure that any potentially relevant items were not missed. Further, where possible, item selection was guided by the previous factor analytic investigations. Given that the SRS-2 has only one item relating to the sensory sensitivity (item 42: seems overly sensitive to sounds, textures, or smiles) and single-item factor is not viable, this item was not included in the analysis. ADI-R has two items that relate to the sensory sensitivity—item 72 (undue general sensitivity to noise) and 73 (abnormal response to specific sensory stimuli) that have been included in only a few of the previous ADI-R factor analyses [8, 15, 27, 47]. Curiously, across all noted studies, items 72 and 73 have loaded onto the IS factor rather than forming a separate sensory sensitivity factor which is most likely due to a combination of the fact that two-item factors are difficult to extract, especially with limited sample sizes, and the fact that sensory sensitivity is highly associated with anxiety [11] which is, in turn, a major driver behind IS [30, 33]. Therefore, we have run a preliminary analysis with items 72 and 73 included and they have loaded onto the IS factor rather than forming separate construct, consistent with noted previous factor analyses. Given the loading onto the IS factor, we have omitted these two items from the models described below. The rationale for excluding these two items was related to the fact that sensory sensitivity and IS are clearly distinct constructs and mixed IS factor could make it more difficult for future studies to interpret the results (e.g., it would not be clear whether association with a particular clinical, genetic or neural correlate is driven by IS or sensory sensitivity aspect). Finally, although ADI-R item 79 (midline hand stereotypies) was not included in the previous studies, we have decided to include item 79 given that this particular RRB does occur in ASD and also occurs (more frequently) across several genetic disorders where ASD is highly prevalent.

The following items were included in the analysis: SRS-2: items 4 (inflexible/rigid behaviors), 20 (unusual sensory interests), 24 (difficulties with changes in routine), 28 (thinks about the same thing), 31 (fixated on certain topics/thoughts), 39 (narrow range of interests), 50 (motor mannerisms) and 61 (inflexible); SCQ: items 7 (says the same thing over and over/insists that someone else says the same thing over and over), 8 (has rituals or has to do things in a particular way or order/insists that others go through said rituals), 11 (unusual interests [e.g., traffic lights]), 12 (interest in parts of a toy/object), 13 (interests unusual in terms of intensity but typical in terms of content), 14 (sensory interests), 15 (motor mannerisms), 16 (complex body movements) and 18 (toy and object that she/he carries around); ADI-R: items 39 (verbal rituals), 67 (unusual preoccupations), 68 (circumscribed interests), 69 (repetitive object use/interest in parts of objects), 70 (compulsions/rituals), 71 (unusual sensory interests), 74 (difficulties with changes in own routines and/or environment), 75 (resistance to trivial changes in the environment not related to the individual), 76 (unusual attachment to objects), 77 (hand and finger mannerisms), 78 (complex mannerisms) and 79 (midline hand movements); ADOS (modules 3 and 4): items D1 (unusual sensory interest in play material/person), D2 (hand a finger and other complex mannerisms), D4 (described above) and D5 (compulsions or rituals).

Data analysis

For each of the measures, item-level data were combined across studies and the total sample was randomly split into exploratory and validation subsamples (SRS-2: Exploratory: N = 8409, Validation: N = 8352; SCQ: Exploratory: N = 8054, Validation: N = 7786; ADI-R: Exploratory: N = 4518, Validation: N = 4467; ADOS: Exploratory: N = 3180, Validation: N = 3134). Exploratory and validation samples did not differ in age and sex distribution. In order to derive factor structure of the RRB domain, a series of latent variable analyses were conducted separately for each of the measures using steps described henceforth. Exploratory application of the Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling (ESEM [48, 49]) used geomin rotation (given that constructs are likely to be correlated [49]). Given the number of items and number of factors derived in previous factor analytic studies across a range of dedicated RRB and general ASD severity measures, models with 1–5 factors were estimated. The number of factors to be retained was guided by a range of recommended fit indices [49,50,51,53]; the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI), the Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) and the standardized root mean square residual (SRMR). The following cut-offs were applied: (i) CFI and TLI values > .90 indicating adequate and > .95 excellent fit; (ii) RMSEA and SRMR values of < .08 indicating adequate and < .06 excellent fit, with the close fit-test with a p value > .05. Because chi-square index tends to be oversensitive to a range of factors including sample size, it was not used. Analyses used polychoric correlations with the weighted least square estimator. Models derived through exploratory models were replicated in the validation sample using the confirmatory application of the ESEM. Model fit was evaluated using noted fit indices. The derived factor solutions were further replicated across sex and age (samples were divided into subsamples aged 2–6, 7–12 and 13 + years). These analyses were run using MPlus [54].

The relationship between derived latent RRB subscales and several variables of interest including chronological age, sex, verbal (VIQ) and non-verbal IQ (NVIQ) and with the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL [55]) was explored using Pearson’s correlation coefficient and one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). All correlations and comparisons were conducted in SPSS [56] and performed through bootstrapping using 5,000 resamples to provide more robust statistics and account for the potential skewness of the data [57]. All comparisons were supplemented with the effect sizes.

Results

The exploratory (referred to as EFA in Table 2) and confirmatory ESEM (referred to as ESEM in Table 2) models are presented in Table 2. As can be seen, across all of the measures, unidimensional models had unsatisfactory fit.

Table 2 Summary of goodness of fit statistics for the tested factor analysis models

SRS-2: The three-factor model had a superior fit to the two-factor model (CFI = .999, TLI = .997, RMSEA = .017, SRMR = .004 vs CFI = .937, TLI = .864, RMSEA = .112, SRMR = .034) and the four-factor model failed to converge. Thus, the three-factor model was retained. Factors were interpreted as repetitive motor behaviors (RMB; 2 items), insistence on sameness (IS; 3 items) and circumscribed interests (CI; 3 items). Confirmatory application of the ESEM demonstrated excellent fit of this model in the validation sample (CFI = .999, TLI = .997, RMSEA = .016, SRMR = .004) and showed excellent fit across sex and age (all CFI ≥ .998, all TLI ≥ .992, all RMSEA ≤ .031, all SRMR ≤ .006). Figure 1a shows factor loadings, standard errors (SE) for each of the loadings (SE) and factor correlations for the confirmatory ESEM for the whole sample. Item loadings for the exploratory three-factor model are provided in Additional file 1: Table S2.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Social Responsiveness Scale correlated 3-factor RRB solution. Solid lines represent factor loadings, and curved lines represent the correlation among factors. Note: CI: Circumscribed Interests; IS: Insistence on Sameness; RMB: Repetitive Motor Behaviors. Item 4 (Shows rigid pattern of behaviors when under stress), item 20 (Unusual sensory interests), item 24 (Difficulties with changes in routine), item 28 (talks about the same thing or topic), item 31 (cannot get his mind of something), item 39 (Unusually narrow range of interests), item 50 (Repetitive motor behaviors such as hand flapping), item 61 (Inflexible), b Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Social Communication Questionnaire correlated 3-factor RRB solution. Solid lines represent factor loadings, and curved lines represent the correlation among factors. Note: IS: insistence on sameness; RMB: repetitive motor behaviors; UI: unusual interests. Item 7 (Says the same thing/insists that others say the same thing over and over), item 8 (Has to do things in a very particular order), item 11 (Interests odd in terms of content [e. g., traffic lights, timetables]), item 12 (Interest in parts of objects [e. g., spinning wheels]), item 13 (interests unusual in intensity but not content/topic), item 14 (unusually interested in sensory stimuli), item 15 (Motor mannerisms/stereotypies [e.g., hand flapping, finger movements]), item 16 (Complex body movements [e.g., spinning, bouncing up and down]), item 18 (Objects to carry around). c Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised correlated 3-factor RRB solution. Solid lines represent factor loadings, and curved lines represent the correlation among factors. Note: IS: insistence on sameness; RMB: repetitive motor behaviors; UI: unusual interests. Item 39 (Verbal rituals), item 68 (Circumscribed interests), item 69 (Repetitive use of/interest in parts of objects), item 70 (Compulsions/rituals), item 71 (Unusual sensory interests), item 74 (Difficulties with minor changes in one’s own routines/environment), item 75 (Resistance to changes in the environment not directly affecting the individual), item 76 (unusual attachment to objects), item 77 (Hand and finger mannerisms), item 78 (Complex mannerisms or body movements), item 79 (Midline hand movements)

SCQ: The three-factor model had a superior fit to the two-factor model (CFI = .999, TLI = .998, RMSEA = .009, SRMR = .011 vs CFI = .980, TLI = .962, RMSEA = .039, SRMR = .030) and the four-factor model failed to converge. Although the two- and three-factor solutions had excellent fit indices, the three-factor solution was retained because additional meaningful factor emerged. Factors were interpreted as repetitive motor behaviors (RMB; 2 items), insistence on sameness (IS; 3 items) and unusual interests (UI; 4 items). The unusual interest factor included sensory interests (item 14), interest in objects/part of objects (item 12) and interests with a narrow focus (e. g. traffic lights or train tables) (item 11). Item 12 (interest in objects/part of objects) loaded on IS and UI factors but was assigned to the IS factor given the higher loading (.44 vs .34). Confirmatory application of the ESEM showed excellent fit of the three-factor model in the validation sample (CFI = .995, TLI = .986, RMSEA = .022, SRMR = .016) and excellent fit across sex and age subgroups (all CFI ≥ .995, all TLI ≥ .985, all RMSEA ≤ .025, all SRMR ≤ .020). Factor loadings, SE and correlations for the confirmatory ESEM are shown in Fig. 1b. Item loadings for the exploratory three-factor model are provided in Additional file 1: Table S3.

ADI-R: Two- and three-factor solutions provided excellent fit (CFI = .973, TLI = .958, RMSEA = .031, SRMR = .029 vs CFI = .983, TLI = .966, RMSEA = .028, SRMR = .024) and four-factor solution failed to converge. In the two-factor solution, RMB and unusual/circumscribed interests items loaded on a single factor. Because RMB and UI emerged as separate factors, the three-factor solution, including RMB, IS and UI, was retained. Three-factor solution was replicated in the validation sample (CFI = .991, TLI = .980, RMSEA = .024, SRMR = .019). The fit across sex and age was excellent (all CFI ≥ .988, all TLI ≥ .973, all RMSEA ≤ .27, all SRMR ≤ .020). Figure 1c shows factor loadings, SE and correlations for the three-factor solution. Item loadings for exploratory three-factor model are provided in Additional file 1: Table S4. Additional file 1: Table S1 shows that three-factor solution provided the best fit for ADI-R ever scores (exploratory sample: CFI = .983, TLI = .966, RMSEA = .026, SRMR = .021; validation sample: CFI = .988, TLI = .973, RMSEA = .026, SRMR = .020).

ADOS Modules 3 and 4: As noted, the unidimensional model had a poor fit in the exploratory sample, however, the two-factor model failed to converge. The unidimensional and two-factor models were conducted in the validation sample with unidimensional model showing poor fit (CFI = .930, TLI = .789, RMSEA = .082, SRMR = .035) and two-factor model failing to converge.

Cross-measure ESEM: we have utilized the cross-measure exploratory ESEM to combine the SRS-2 and ADI-R items to investigate whether additional RRB factors would emerge. Given that the similarity between the ADI-R and the SCQ, the dichotomous nature of the SCQ scoring and that the available SCQ data were from the lifetime rather than the current form, the analysis focused on the SRS-2 and ADI-R. Where identical items existed across the SRS-2 and ADI-R, only one of the items was included to avoid problems with the convergence and fit. Finally, SRS-2 item 42 and ADI-R items 72 and 73 capturing different aspects of sensory sensitivity were included in the analysis. Three-factor solution showed adequate fit (CFI = .949, TLI = .923, RMSEA = .049, SRMR = .039) and four- (CFI = .979, TLI = .959, RMSEA = .036, SRMR = .029) and five-factor (CFI = .992, TLI = .984, RMSEA = .023, SRMR = .021) solutions showed excellent fit. Factor loadings for four- and five-factor solutions are showed in Additional file 1: Tables S5 and S6. RMB, IS, UI and CI factors emerged across both four- and five-solutions; the main distinction was that while in the four-factor solution three sensory sensitivity items (SRS-2 item 42 and ADI-R items 72 and 73) loaded onto the IS factor, in the five-factor solution these items formed a separate sensory sensitivity factor. Therefore, the five-factor solution was considered more optimal.

Table 3 shows the patterns of associations between the SRS-2, the SCQ and the ADI-R RRB scores and corresponding RBS-R subscales scores. RBS-R data were available for N = 4097 participants with the SRS-2, N = 3489 with the ADI-R and N = 1602 with the SCQ data. Using Fisher r-to-z transformation on the Pearson correlation values, we examined the convergent and divergent validity of the RMB, IS CI and UI scores across the SRS-2, the SCQ and the ADI-R. The association between RMB scores with the RBS-R Stereotypy subscale was significantly stronger when compared to other subscales in SRS-2 (Fisher r-to-z Z range: 14.36–23.83, all p < .001), in SCQ (Z range: 6.69–11.56, all p < .001), and in ADI-R (Z range: 11.41–16.59, all p < .001). Significantly stronger associations were observed between IS scores with the RBS-R Ritualistic/Sameness subscale when compared to other subscales in SRS-2 (Fisher r-to-z Z range: 12.52–18.08, all p < .001), in SCQ (Z range: 1.79, p = .037–6.98, p < .001), and ADI-R (Z range: 5.46–11.72, all p < .001). SRS-2 CI score showed a significantly stronger association with the RBS-R Restricted subscale when compared to the Stereotypy subscale (Fisher r-to-z Z range: 11.40, p < .001) but similar strength of the relationship as with the RBS-R Ritualistic/Sameness subscale. UI subscale of both the SCQ and the ADI-R showed significantly higher associations with the RBS-R Restricted Behaviors subscale than with the RBS-R Ritualistic/Sameness subscale (SCQ: Fisher r-to-z Z = 5.34, p < .001; ADI-R: Z = 4.69, p < .001); however, no differences were observed in terms of association with the Stereotypy subscale.

Table 3 Pattern of associations between identified RRB factors with other individual characteristics

As can be seen from Table 3, younger age was associated with higher RMB and lower IS factors across all scales; however, although significant, associations were weak. IQ data were available for 2742 participants with the SRS-2, for 2755 participants with the ADI-R, and for 1178 participants with the SCQ. The CBCL data were available for 3532 participants with the SRS-2, for 3402 participants with the ADI-R, and for 1240 participants with the SCQ. Verbal (VIQ) and non-verbal IQ (NVIQ) scores were inversely correlated with RMB across all measures and with UI on SCQ and ADI-R. The associations between VIQ and NVIQ with IS were of low magnitude. CBCL anxiety scores were positively associated with the SRS-2 and ADI-R IS scores as well as with SRS-2 CI and RMB scores. The IS-anxiety correlation between SRS-2 IS (r = .40) was significantly higher than RMB-anxiety correlation (r = .13); Fisher r-to-z Z = 12, p < .001. CBCL externalizing scores were positively associated with IS and CI scores across all measures and with the RMB scores on the SRS-2 (but not on SCQ and ADI-R). Mean scores were significantly higher in males compared to female participants across all RRB subscales (Table 4).

Table 4 Sex comparison across RRB factors

Discussion

The current study utilized large, publicly available databases to explore the capacity of the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS-2), the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ), the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) to capture distinct constructs within the restricted and repetitive behaviors (RRB) domain in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). With the exception of the ADOS, a three-factor structure emerged as the best fit to data for the other three measures. The three-factor model was confirmed in the second round of analyses and performed well across sex and age. Repetitive-motor behaviors (RMB) and insistence on sameness (IS) factors were consistently identified. However, the domain of circumscribed interests (CI) and unusual interests (UI) was not uniform. The SRS-2 recognized circumscribed interests with items capturing interests unusual in terms of their focus, intensity and inflexibility but typical/age-appropriate in terms of their content/subject (e.g., particular animals, fictional characters or content topic in children). By contrast, the unusual interests factor encompassing items such as fascination with sensory stimuli, fascination with parts of the object and interests unusual in terms of their content/subject (timetables, traffic lights) emerged from the SCQ and ADI-R. Given the relationship between RMB and UI factors, it will be important for future studies to further evaluate their distinctiveness Analyses of ADOS modules 3 and 4 suggest that more fine-grained RRB models are not viable.

Content of RMB and IS factors identified across the SRS-2, SCQ and ADI-R conceptually resemble the content within cognate subscales of the RBS-R as well as other instruments including the RBQ, the RBQ-2, and the CRI-R. For example, the RMB and IS factors derived here showed significantly stronger associations with the corresponding RBS-R subscales (for instance, RMB subscale was associated more strongly with RBS-R Stereotypy subscale than with RBS-R Ritualistic/Sameness and Restricted subscales). Furthermore, ADI-R RMB factor derived here encompassed items 77 and 78 that consistently loaded on RMB factor across a range of previous ADI-R factor analytic explorations (e.g., [8, 10, 26, 28, 40]). RMB factor derived in our study further included item 79 that was not been included in previous ADI-R investigations. With the exception of sensory hyper-sensitivity items, ADI-R IS factor identified in this study resembled the IS factor derived in several previous ADI-R factor analyses [8, 10, 26, 28]. Repetitive use/interest in parts of objects (item 69) and unusual sensory interests items (item 71) that loaded onto the RMB factor across several previous ADI-R studies [8, 26, 28] loaded onto a separate UI ADI-R factor in our investigation. This was most likely due to the large sample size enabling more fine-grained factors to emerge. UI factor derived in our analysis bears a strong resemblance to the ADI-R Sensory Interests factor derived by Frazier and Hardan [2017] and to the complex stereotypies factor derived by Smith and colleagues [43]. In addition, UI factor derived in our study resembled the Unusual Sensory Interests factor from the RBQ-2 identified by Leekam et al. (2007). ADI-R item 68 that specifically refers to interests that are unusual in terms of their inflexibility has loaded onto the IS factor in our study. This finding is consistent with several previous ADI-R factor analyses (e.g., [8, 38, 43, 47]) and most likely due to the fact that lack of flexibility is a common element across IS and this CI subtype. If additional ADI-R items capturing CI subtype that is characterized by inflexibility but typical content were available, it is likely that they would load onto separate factors, as was the case with the SRS-2. This was indeed confirmed by the findings from the cross-measure ESEM.

The secondary aim of the current study was to characterize the relationship between derived RRB factors with other individual characteristics. Associations with age, cognitive functioning, anxiety and externalizing symptoms suggested somewhat distinctive patterns. Higher RMB and UI were associated with younger age and lower VIQ and NVIQ. IS scores across all measures and CI scores on the SRS-2 were directly correlated with older age and to a lesser degree but in the same direction with VIQ and NVIQ. Applying Fisher’s r to z transformation anxiety was more strongly associated with IS and CI factors across the ADI-R and SRS-2, and RBS-R than with RMB subscale across these measures. This observation is consistent with prior studies from our group [11, 33, 59, 60] and others [30,31,32]. The association of anxiety with IS implies that successful treatment of anxiety might also benefit IS [33]. Externalizing symptoms were positively correlated with the SRS-2 subscales, especially the IS subscale. The correlations of externalizing symptoms with subscales on all other measures were weakly positive. Finally, the examination of sex differences showed that mean scores on all RRB subscales were greater across all measures in males compared to females.

The negative association between RMB with age and IQ identified aligns with Turner’s [17] conceptualization of lower-order RRB and with findings from longitudinal and cross-sectional studies in ASD [8, 26, 58] and community samples [61]. The positive relationship between age with IS and CI (as measured by the SRS-2) and weaker relationship with IQ is also in line with a range of previous studies [5, 10, 29]. The observed association between anxiety and more severe IS is supported by several previous studies [11, 30, 33, 59, 60]. Moreover, the positive association between anxiety and externalizing problems with IS reported here is also consistent with other studies [31, 62]. In normative development, emergence and increase in IS occur along with the emergence of normative fears [63]. This suggests that IS may serve as an early form of self-regulation by exerting control on the environment, limiting unpredictability, and reducing normative fears [33, 64, 65]. With the development of more mature forms of self-regulation, IS is reduced and therefore transitory [61]. However, given the impairments in self-regulation in ASD, children with ASD may continue to rely on IS to manage anxiety [33, 61]. Reliance on IS, ironically, does not appear to be effective in the long-term and might even exacerbate anxiety [33]. In addition, it has been suggested that over-reliance on IS might further compromise the development of self-regulation due to the restrictive nature of IS and limited opportunities for learning and development [33]. Furthermore, deviations from routines can exacerbate anxiety and lead to meltdowns and destructive and aggressive behavioral patterns, which is in line with the positive association between IS and externalizing problems reported here. The relationship between CI with anxiety and externalizing problems found in our study is likely to be a consequence of individuals not being able to engage with their preferred interests and activities. For instance, one of the common themes that emerged from a qualitative study by Halim et al. [62] that investigated presentation and triggers for anxiety in ASD was that prevention of engaging in both preferred interests and routines could be a cause of anxiety, stress and meltdowns. Although findings reported here provide further support to the previously suggested link between IS and CI with internalizing and externalizing problems, and the potential mediating and/or moderating role of impairments in self-regulation, these findings are based on cross-sectional and correlational findings. Therefore, future research using longitudinal designs, especially focusing on early stages of development when these behaviors start to emerge, is needed to disentangle the nature of the reported relationships and elucidate whether these symptom domains are mechanistically linked (e. g. elevated anxiety gives rise to IS) or they are underpinned by a common underlying cognitive and neurobiological mechanisms such as, for instance, impairments in different facets of self-regulation or imbalance between habit and goal-directed systems. These insights are particularly crucial given the impairing nature of both IS and anxiety in ASD, and recent longitudinal findings by Baribeau and colleagues [66] suggesting their stability or even increase during early and middle childhood in this population and findings suggesting that anxiety in ASD tends to be stable across the life-span [68].

Limitations

The five datasets used here afforded a large as well as wide distribution of the RRB item scores. However, RBS-R scores used for establishing convergent and divergent validity of the derived factor scores were only available for a portion of the sample. Similarly, IQ and CBCL data were available only for a portion of participants. However, the sample size available to explore the pattern of relationships between derived RRB factor scores with other individual characteristics such as IQ, anxiety and externalizing problems was an order of magnitude larger than samples used in previous investigations. RMB SRS-2 factor contained only 2 items, and the majority of other derived factors had 3 items. Despite this limited item coverage, it has been shown that two, or even single-item indicators can be reliable [67, 69, 71] and this is further supported by the stability of the SRS-2 RMB factor (and other factors containing 3 items) across exploratory and confirmatory subsamples, as well as across sex and age subgroups. In addition, both the SCQ and the ADI-R were developed as screening and/or diagnostic instruments rather than for quantifying individual variability in the symptom severity and are therefore likely not sensitive enough to capture subtle symptom expressions observed below the diagnostic threshold, across a range of neurodevelopmental and neuropsychiatric disorders and in early normative development where distinct RMB and IS behaviors commonly occur (as discussed above). Therefore, further work is needed in order to appraise the viability of using the SCQ and ADI-R for this purpose. Ideally, quantitative comprehensive instruments optimized for capturing subtle variations in symptom expression and severity should be used to further our understanding of the phenomenological and mechanistic continuities and discontinuities in RRB across normative and atypical development, including ASD. Finally, it is possible that a small portion of participants overlapped across datasets, however, given the large sample size, overlapping cases are unlikely to have any impact on results.

Conclusions

In summary, our findings demonstrate the capacity and the limitations of the SRS-2, SCQ and ADI-R to measure RRB in individuals with ASD. These measures were not specifically developed to capture distinct RRB domains; however, they are widely used in research and clinical practice and have been collected in several large datasets that are available to the research community. Therefore, the RRB factors derived here present an important resource for both re-analysis of the currently existing data and future investigations utilizing these measures. In particular, IS factor is relatively robust across all measures, encompassing three items for both the SRS-2 and the SCQ and five items for the ADI-R. The ADI-R RMB factor is also relatively robust; however, this factor is captured by only two SCQ and SRS-2 items. Finally, CI factor is only captured by the SRS-2, and the UI factor encompassing items such as fascination with sensory stimuli, fascination with parts of the object and interests unusual in terms of their content/subject emerged across the SCQ and the ADI-R, but not the SRS-2. CI and UI factors showed distinct correlations with factors such as age and IQ and therefore should be considered as separate in future investigations. Described factors showed good convergence with corresponding factors from a widely used dedicated RRB measure (RSB-R) and expected pattern of predicted external correlates (e.g., age, FSIQ, externalizing and internalizing symptoms). Therefore, RRB factors derived here can be used across both already collected data sets and newly planned studies to further our understanding of their relationship with a range of demographic, developmental and clinical correlates. Although two-item factors can be reliable, they nevertheless have a limited range which can somewhat limit their use in longitudinal and neurobiological research and although factors derived here can be used for such purposes, findings should be interpreted as preliminary, forming an initial first step towards future studies that would utilize detailed RRB instruments. Findings reported here provide important information for the development of future RRB questionnaire and interview measures. More specifically, our analysis demonstrated that the SCQ, SRS-2 and ADI-R provide a very limited sampling of sensory domains and very poor capture of the CI domain. This limitation is not specific to instruments analyzed here but also extends to dedicated RRB instruments such as the RBS-R and CY-BOCS-ASD. Although the RBQ-2 and the CRI-R have several sensory related items, they have not emerged as a separate factor in the factor analyses published thus far. Findings emphasizing the importance of distinguishing between interests that are unusual in terms of focus/flexibility and/or intensity rather than content and interests that are unusual in terms of content is particularly informative for the development of new instruments. Furthermore, the fact that sensory sensitivity factor emerged in the cross-measure ESEM that combined the SRS-2 and ADI-R items supports the viability of the sensory sensitivity as a separate domain linked to RRB. Additionally, new instruments should provide an in-depth sampling of each separate domain, with 10–15 items per subscale in order to provide an adequate range for different research and clinical purposes, in particular person-centered profiling and treatment tracking. New instruments will need to be designed and refined based on the gold standard measurement development standards (e.g., Patient-Reported Outcome Measurement Information System [PROMIS; 70]) and validated using the state-of-the-art psychometric approaches. Finally, our findings suggest that although ADOS is a valid and reliable diagnostic instrument, the coverage of RRB does not allow a fine-grained assessment of RRB domains. This finding emphasizes the need to develop new, dedicated observational assessment protocols specifically focused on RRB.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ADI-R:

Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ASD:

Autism Spectrum Disorder

CI:

Circumscribed Interests

CRI:

Childhood Routines Inventory

ESEM:

Exploratory Structural Equation Modeling

FSIQ:

Full-scale Intelligence Quotient

IS:

Insistence on Sameness

RBS-R:

Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised

RMB:

Repetitive Motor Behaviors

RRB:

Restricted and Repetitive Behaviors

SCQ:

Social Communication Questionnaire

SRS-2:

Social Responsiveness Scale

UI:

Unusual Interests

References

  1. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child. 1943;2:217–53.

    Google Scholar 

  2. Boyd BA, McDonough SG, Bodfish JW. Evidence-based behavioral interventions for repetitive behaviors in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2012;42:1236–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Harrop C, McBee M, Boyd BA. How are child restricted and repetitive behaviors associated with caregiver stress over time? A parallel process multilevel growth model. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(5):1773–83.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Leekam SR, Prior MR, Uljarević M. Restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders: a review of research in the last decade. Psychol Bull. 2011;137(4):562–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. South M, Ozonoff S, McMahon WM. Repetitive behaviour profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2005;35:145–58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Honey E, Rodgers J, McConachie H. Measurement of restricted and repetitive behaviour in children with autism spectrum disorder: selecting a questionnaire or interview. Res Autism Spect Disord. 2012;6(2):757–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Scahill L, Aman MG, Lecavalier L, Halladay AK, Bishop SL, Bodfish JW, et al. Measuring repetitive behaviors as a treatment endpoint in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. 2015;9:38–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Bishop SL, Hus V, Duncan A, Huerta M, Gotham K, Pickles A, Lord C. Subcategories of restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(6):1287–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Bodfish JW, Symons FJ, Parker DE, Lewis MH. Varieties of repetitive behavior in autism: comparisons to mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:237–43.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Lam KSL, Bodfish JW, Piven J. Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behaviour in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008;49(11):1193–200.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Lidstone J, Uljarević M, Sullivan J, Rodgers J, McConachie H, Freeston M, et al. Relations among restricted and repetitive behaviors, anxiety and sensory features in children with autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spect Disord. 2014;8(2):82–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. The Social Responsiveness Scale Manual, Second Edition (SRS-2). Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  13. Rutter M, Bailey A, Lord C. The social communication questionnaire manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S, Gotham K, Bishop S. Autism diagnostic observation schedule: ADOS-2. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Honey E, McConachie H, Randle V, Shearer H, Le Couteur A. One-year change in repetitive behaviours in young children with communication disorders including autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1439–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Prior M, Macmillan MB. Maintenance of sameness in children with Kanner’s syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 1973;3:154–67.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Turner M. Annotation: Repetitive behavior in autism: A review of psychological research. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1999;40:839–49.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Bodfish JW, Symons FW, Lewis MH. The Repetitive Behavior Scale. Western Carolina Center Research Reports; 1999.

  19. Turner, M. Repetitive behaviour and cognitive functioning in autism. Unpublished doctoral thesis. University of Cambridge, UK; 1999.

  20. Barrett SL, Uljarević M, Baker EK, Richdale AL, Jones CRG, Leekam SR. The Adult Repetitive Behaviours Questionnaire-2 (RBQ-2A): A self-report measure of restricted and repetitive behaviours. J Autism Dev Disord. 2015;45:3680–92.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Barrett SL, Uljarević M, Jones CRG, Leekam SR. Assessing subtypes of restricted and repetitive behaviour using the Adult Repetitive Behaviour Questionnaire2 in autistic adults. Mol Autism. 2018;9(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Leekam SR, Tandos J, McConachie H, Meins E, Parkinson K, Wright C, et al. Repetitive behaviors in typically developing 2-year-olds. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(11):1131–2113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Evans DW, Leckman JF, Carter A, Reznick JS, Henshaw D, King RA, Pauls D. Ritual, habit and perfectionism: the prevalence and development of compulsive-like behavior in normal young children. Child Dev. 1997;68(58–68):8.

    Google Scholar 

  24. Evans DW, Uljarević M, Lusk L, Loth E, Frazier T. Development of two dimensional measures of restricted and repetitive behavior in parents and children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56:51–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Scahill L, Dimitropoulos A, McDougle CJ, Aman MG, Feurer ID, McCracken JT, et al. Children’s Yale-Brown Obsessive Compulsive Scale in autism spectrum disorder: component structure and correlates of symptom checklist. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. 2014;53(1):97–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Cuccaro ML, Shao Y, Grubber J, Slifer M, Wolpert CM, Donnelly SL, et al. Factor analysis of restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-R. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2003;34:3–17.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12:247–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Bishop SL, Richler J, Lord C. Association between restricted and repetitive behaviors and nonverbal IQ in children with autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. 2006;12(4–5):247–67.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Hus V, Pickles A, Cook EH, Risi S, Lord C. Using the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised to increase phenotypic homogeneity in genetic studies of autism. Biol Psychiat. 2007;61:438–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Gotham K, Bishop SL, Hus V, Huerta M, Lund S, Buja A, Krieger A, Lord C. Exploring the relationship between anxiety and insistence on sameness in autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2013;6(1):33–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Scahill L, Lecavalier L, Schultz RT, Evans AN, Maddox B, et al. Development of the parent-rated anxiety scale for youth with autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;58(9):887–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Spiker MA, Lin CE, Van Dyke M, Wood JJ. Restricted interests and anxiety in children with autism. Autism. 2012;16(3):306–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Uljarević M, Richdale A, Cai R-Y, Evans DW, Leekam SR. Inter-relationship between insistence on sameness, effortful control and anxiety in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Mol Autism. 2017;8:36.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Langen M, Durston S, Kas MJH, vanEngeland H, Staal WG. The neurobiology of repetitive behavior:and men. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):356–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Cannon DS, Miller JS, Robinson RJ, Villalobos ME, Wahmhoff NK, Allen-Brady K, et al. Genome-wide linkage analyses of two repetitive behavior phenotypes in Utah pedigrees with autism spectrum disorders. Mol Autism. 2010;1:3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  36. Liu X-Q, Georgiades S, Duku E, Thompson A, Devlin B, Cook EH, et al. Identification of genetic loci underlying the phenotypic constructs of autism spectrum disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolescent Psychiatry. 2013;50:687–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Shao Y, Cuccaro M, Hauser E, Raiford KL, Menold MM, Wolpert CM, et al. Fine mapping of autistic disorder to chromosome 15q11-q13 by use of phenotypic subtypes. Am J Hum Genet. 2003;72:539–48.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Tao Y, Gao H, Ackerman B, Guo W, Saffen D, Shugart YY. Evidence for contribution of common genetic variants within chromosome 8p21.2–8p21.1 to restricted and repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. BMC Genom. 2016;17:163.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Silverman J, Smith C, Schmeidler J, Hollander E, Lawlor B, Fitzgerald M, Galvin P, et al. Symptom domains in autism and related conditions: evidence for familiality. Am J Med Genet. 2002;114:64–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Bryson S, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Mahoney W, et al. Investigating the structure of the restricted, repetitive behaviours and interests domain of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2006;47:582–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Uljarevic M, Evans DW, Alvares GA, Whitehouse AJO. Short report: relationship between restricted and repetitive behaviours in children with autism spectrum disorder and their parents. Molecular Autism. 2016; 7:26.

  42. Le Couteur A, Lord C, Rutter M. The Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Smith CJ, Lang CM, Kryzak L, Reichenberg A, Hollander E, Silverman M. Familial associations of intense preoccupations, an empirical factor of the restricted, repetitive behaviors and interests domain of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50(8):982–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Alexander LM, Escalera J, Ai L, Andreotti C, Febre K, Mangone A, et al. An open resource for transdiagnostic research in pediatric mental health and learning disorders. Sci Data. 2017;4:170181.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Fischbach GD, Lord C. The Simons Simplex Collection: a resource for identification of autism genetic risk factors. Neuron. 2010;68(2):192–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Geschwind DH, Sowinski J, Lord C, Iversen P, Shestack J, Jones P, Ducat L, Spence SJ, AGRE Steering Committee. The autism genetic resource exchange: a resource for the study of autism and related neuropsychiatric conditions. Am J Hum Genet. 2001;69:463–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  47. Hundley RJ, Shui A, Malow BA. Relationship between subtypes of restricted and repetitive behaviors and sleep disturbance in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(11):3448–57.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Asparouhov T, Muthén B. Exploratory structural equation modeling. Struct Equ Model. 2009;16:397–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Marsh HW, Morin AJS, Parker P, Kaur G. Exploratory structural equation modeling: an integration of the best features of exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2014;10:85–110.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural equation models. Psychol Bull. 1998;107:238–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Cheung GW, Rensvold RB. Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model. 2002;9(2):233–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Kass RE, Raftery AE. Bayes factors. J Am Stat Assoc. 1995;90(430):773–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Sclove LS. Application of model-selection criteria to some problems in multivariate analysis. Psychometrika. 1987;52:333–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus user’s guide. 5th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA preschool forms & profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont, Research Center for Children, Youth and Families; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  56. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS statistics for windows (Version 24.0) [Computer Program]. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp; 2016.

  57. Efron B, Tibshirani R. An introduction to the bootstrap. Boca Raton: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 1993.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  58. Richler J, Huerta M, Bishop SL, Lord C. Developmental trajectories of restricted and repetitive behaviors and interests in children with autism spectrum disorders. Dev Psychopathol. 2010;22(1):55–69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  59. Uljarevic M, Evans DW. Relationship between repetitive behaviour and fear across normative development, autism spectrum disorder, and down syndrome. Autism Res. 2018;10(3):502–7.

  60. Uljarevic M, McCabe KL, Angkustsiri K, Simon TJ, Hardan AY. Interrelationship between cognitive control, anxiety, and restricted and repetitive behaviors in children with 22q11 2. Deletion syndrome. Autism Res. 2019;12(12):1737–44.

  61. Uljarević M, Arnott B, Carrington SJ, Meins E, Fernyhough C, McConachie H, et al. Development of restricted and repetitive behaviors from 15 to 77 months: Stability of two distinct subtypes? Dev Psychol. 2017;53:1859–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Halim A, Richdale A, Uljarević M. Exploring the nature of anxiety in young adults on the autism spectrum: a qualitative study. Res Autism Spectrum Disord. 2018;55:25–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Gullone E. The development of normal fear: a century of research. Clin Psychol Rev. 2000;20(4):429–51.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Evans DW, Gray FL, Leckman JF. The rituals, fears and phobias of young children: Insights from development, psychopathology and neurobiology. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 1999;29(4):261–76.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Gesell A, Ames LB, Ilg FL. Infant and the child in culture today. New York: Harper and Row; 1974.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Baribeau DA, Vigod S, Pullenayegum E, Kerns CM, Mirenda P, Smith IM, et al. Co-occurring trajectories of anxiety and insistence on sameness behaviour in autism spectrum disorder. Br J Psychiatry; 2020 (early online).

  67. Gosling SD, Rentfrow PJ, Swann WB. A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. J Res Pers. 2003;37(6):504–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  68. Uljarevic M, Hedley D, Rose-Foley K, Magiati I, Cai R.-Y, Dissanayake C, Richdale A, Trollor J. Anxiety and depression from adolescence to old age in autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;50(9):3155–65

  69. Strauss GP, Nuñes A, Ahmed AO, Barchard KA, Granholm E, Kirkpatrick B, et al. The latent structure of negative symptoms in schizophrenia. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(12):1271–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. PROMIS® Validity Standards Committee on behalf of the PROMIS® Network. PROMIS® instrument development and psychometric evaluation scientific standards. 2013; 1–72.

  71. Uljarevic M, Frazier TW, Phillips JW, Jo B, Littlefield S, Hardan AY. Mapping the research domain criteria social processes constructs to the social responsiveness scale. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2020;59(11):1252–63.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are sincerely indebted to the generosity of the families and patients who contributed their time and effort.

Funding

The study was supported by grant R21MH121876-01 (Hardan & Jo) by the National Institute of Mental Health. Dr. Uljarević is currently supported by the Australian Research Council Discovery Early Career Researcher Award (DE180100632).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MU and AYH designed the study. MU and AYH wrote the manuscript. MU conducted the data analysis. All authors critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Mirko Uljarević.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This research study was approved by the Stanford University Institutional Review Board. This was a secondary data analysis of existing, publicly available databases, each of which encompassed a range of individual studies. Individual studies employed distinct recruitment and consenting process, inclusionary and exclusionary criteria, and incentivization approaches and strategies. All participants implicitly or explicitly consented for their anonymized data to be added to the database and publicly shared.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Table S1. Social Responsiveness Scale item loadings for exploratory models. Table S2. Social Communication Questionnaire item loadings for exploratory models. Table S3. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised item loadings for exploratory models. Table S4. Summary of Goodness of Fit Indices for Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised Models based on “Ever” scores. Table S5. Cross-measure Exploratory Structural Equation Modelling factor loadings for four-factor solution. Table S6. Cross-measure Exploratory Structural Equation Modelling factor loadings for five-factor solution

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Uljarević, M., Jo, B., Frazier, T.W. et al. Using the big data approach to clarify the structure of restricted and repetitive behaviors across the most commonly used autism spectrum disorder measures. Molecular Autism 12, 39 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00419-9

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00419-9

Keywords