- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Atypical development of white matter microstructure of the corpus callosum in males with autism: a longitudinal investigation
Molecular Autism volume 6, Article number: 15 (2015)
Abstract
Background
The corpus callosum is the largest white matter structure in the brain, and it is the most consistently reported to be atypical in diffusion tensor imaging studies of autism spectrum disorder. In individuals with typical development, the corpus callosum is known to undergo a protracted development from childhood through young adulthood. However, no study has longitudinally examined the developmental trajectory of corpus callosum in autism past early childhood.
Methods
The present study used a cohort sequential design over 9Â years to examine age-related changes of the corpus callosum in 100 males with autism and 56 age-matched males with typical development from early childhood (when autism can first be reliably diagnosed) to mid-adulthood (after development of the corpus callosum has been completed) (3 to 41Â years of age).
Results
The group with autism demonstrated a different developmental trajectory of white matter microstructure in the anterior corpus callosum’s (genu and body) fractional anisotropy, which suggests atypical brain maturation in these regions in autism. When analyses were broken down by age group, atypical developmental trajectories were present only in the youngest participants (10 years of age and younger). Significant main effects for group were found in terms of decreased fractional anisotropy across all three subregions of the corpus callosum (genu, body, and splenium) and increased mean diffusivity, radial diffusivity, and axial diffusivity in the posterior corpus callosum.
Conclusions
These longitudinal results suggest atypical early childhood development of the corpus callosum microstructure in autism that transitions into sustained group differences in adolescence and adulthood. This pattern of results provides longitudinal evidence consistent with a growing number of published studies and hypotheses regarding abnormal brain connectivity across the life span in autism.
Background
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a genetically complex neurodevelopmental disorder that may be marked by atypical functional connectivity within and between particular neural networks and regions in the brain [1-4]. Atypical functional connectivity may implicate white matter, which contains bundles of axons that allow for fast and efficient neuronal communication [5]. The largest white matter tract is the corpus callosum, which facilitates interaction between the left and right hemispheres across multiple lobes. Studies in individuals with callosal agenesis or with a surgically severed corpus callosum suggest that the corpus callosum is involved in a diversity of functions but may be particularly involved in cognitive processes that require the integration of complex information (for a review, see [6]). Because persons with ASD have been shown to demonstrate difficulty with higher order or complex information processing tasks [7-9], the corpus callosum may be a key area of study in this disorder, and within-group corpus callosum differences may be able to help disentangle aspects of the clinical heterogeneity commonly observed in ASD.
Multiple sources of evidence suggest corpus callosum atypicalities in persons with ASD. The corpus callosum was one of the first brain structures observed to be abnormal in in vivo neuroimaging studies of autism [10-14]. Decreased mean size of the corpus callosum, especially the anterior aspects [15], is one of the most replicated structural imaging results of case-control studies of ASD (for a meta-analysis, see [16]). Functional connectivity measures have suggested decreased interhemispheric connectivity in young children, adolescents, and adults with ASD [2,17-19]. Because the corpus callosum is composed predominately of parallel myelinated axon bundles in primates [20], it is well suited for investigation using diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) [21] to examine the microstructural integrity of the corpus callosum in persons with ASD. DTI is based upon a Gaussian model of water diffusion in tissue and provides a description of white matter microstructure through the tensor measures, including the fractional anisotropy (FA), mean diffusivity (MD), radial diffusivity (RD), and axial diffusivity (AD). These DTI measures provide complimentary information about the microstructure of the corpus callosum. For example, MD is the average diffusivity over all directions and is inversely related to the density of cellular membranes. RD is defined as the shortest eigenvector of the diffusion tensor (perpendicular to the corpus callosum white matter fibers) and has been associated with myelin abnormalities [22,23]. AD is defined as the longest eigenvector of the diffusion tensor (parallel to the corpus callosum fibers) and is affected in axonal injury and sensitive to cytoskeletal features [24]. FA is a normalized measure of variance among the lengths of all three eigenvectors of the diffusion tensor. An FA close to zero represents isotropic diffusion with equally long eigenvectors, and an FA of one represents highly directional diffusion with maximal elongation of one eigenvector compared to the others.
FA has been the most common measure of white matter microstructure used in DTI studies of autism [25], but it is encouraged to supplement measures of FA with measures of MD, RD, and AD in order to more accurately characterize white matter microstructure [26]. White matter microstructure and corresponding DTI values can be affected by changes in myelination, axonal density, axonal degeneration, axonal packing, neuroinflammation, fiber crossing, fiber curving, or fiber branching. The specific pattern of group differences across FA, MD, RD, and AD may be more indicative of one type of white matter atypicality than another [24], suggesting the need to examine the pattern of results across all of the available metrics.
A number of recent studies have used DTI methods in cross-sectional examinations of white matter in ASD, and one of the most consistent findings is decreased FA and increased MD of the corpus callosum in ASD (for a review, see [25]). In addition, a subset of these studies has shown significantly increased RD but not AD in ASD [27-32]. Increased RD may reflect a thinner myelin sheath of the axons [33,22,23,34], which suggests that the axons in the white matter tracts of the corpus callosum are less myelinated in persons with ASD.
Despite the consistency of these results, there are a handful of studies that have found either increased FA or no group differences in corpus callosum microstructure [32,35-37]. Two of these studies investigated very young subjects (that is, 1.8-to-3.3-year-old children in [35] and 1.5-to-5.8-year-old children in [32]), and it is possible that this discrepancy in findings may be due to developmental effects very early in life. Indeed, in individuals with typical development, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies suggest that white matter tracts, including the corpus callosum, tend to increase in FA and decrease in MD from childhood into adulthood [38-43], and if there is a differing developmental trajectory of the white matter integrity in ASD, the detection of group differences may be more or less likely at particular ages.
Only longitudinal studies can satisfactorily examine developmental trajectories, and to date, only one longitudinal DTI study in ASD has been conducted in very young children. Wolff and colleagues [44] found that the high-risk infants who went on to develop ASD demonstrated significantly greater FA of the corpus callosum at 6Â months of age, no difference in FA at 12Â months of age, and significantly decreased FA at 24Â months of age compared to high-risk infants who did not go on to develop ASD. These results suggest that young children who develop ASD may undergo distinct development of the corpus callosum microstructure during infancy. However, studies of older children and adults with ASD are needed. In typically developing individuals, the corpus callosum undergoes an uneven and prolonged developmental trajectory that likely continues through adolescence [39-43]. This protracted development of the corpus callosum through childhood and adolescence and indications of atypical corpus callosum structure in ASD demonstrate the need for a longitudinal examination of the corpus callosum in children, adolescents, and adults. Understanding the development of the corpus callosum in ASD may be critical, given that atypical development of this structure could have cascading effects on other aspects of brain development, including early cortical development and the formation of synchronous neuronal assemblies [45,46].
The present study extends the cross-sectional study of Alexander and colleagues [27] to longitudinally examine DTI age-related changes of the corpus callosum of 100 individuals with ASD and 56 age-matched individuals with typical development from early childhood (when ASDs can first be reliably diagnosed) to mid-adulthood (after development of the corpus callosum has been completed) (3.0 to 41.8Â years old).
Methods
Design
The study employed a cohort sequential design (that is, an accelerated longitudinal design) [47], which simultaneously measured individual longitudinal changes in DTI measurements across multiple age cohorts. Each participant was imaged and clinical measures were collected one to four times over a 9-year period (see Figure 1). Fifty-six participants had four scans (41 ASD and 15 typically developing), 36 participants had three scans (22 ASD and 14 typically developing, 38 participants had two scans (20 ASD and 18 typically developing), and 26 participants had one scan (17 ASD and 9 typically developing). The average interscan interval was 2.6 years.
Participants
The study protocol was approved by both the University of Utah and University of Wisconsin-Madison Institutional Review Boards. Consent was obtained for all participants, and for participants under the age of 18 years, both parental consent and participant assent were obtained. All TDC and the majority of the ASD participants were community ascertained. Participants for this study included 100 males with ASD and 56 males with typical development between the ages of 3 and 41. These participants were selected from participants in the broader longitudinal neuroimaging study (110 ASD and 78 TDC). Participants who met criteria for ASD or TDC at enrollment but who were found to be atypical at future time points (for example, one ASD participant developed a significant medical disorder affecting the brain; several TDC participants developed depression), participants who did not have an age match in the other group, or participants whose scans did not meet quality control checks (see ‘Imaging protocol’ below) were excluded. Time 1 data from a subset of these participants were reported in the study by Alexander and colleagues [27]. The participants with ASD were diagnosed based on Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised [48], Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale (ADOS) [49], Diagnostic Statistical Manual-IV-TR [50], and ICD-10 criteria. Participants with ASD were included in the present study if they met criteria for a lifetime diagnosis of autistic disorder, Asperger’s syndrome, or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified (89% met full criteria for autistic disorder). Exclusion criteria included medical causes of ASD (determined by patient history, physical exam, fragile-X testing, and karyotype), history of severe head injury, hypoxia-ischemia, seizure disorder, and other neurological disorders. ASD is thought to be a highly heterogeneous disorder [51], and the heterogeneity in the IQ domain of our ASD sample can be seen in the ranges of scores in Table 1. Typically developing control participants were confirmed as having typical development through history, the ADOS, IQ testing, and neuropsychological and standardized psychiatric assessment. All participants had English as their first language and were verbal at the time of testing. Forty-nine participants with ASD reported being on a centrally active medication (that is, stimulant, antidepressant, neuroleptic, atypical neuroleptic, or anti-anxiety medication) during at least one of the scans (38 at time 1, 31 at time 2, 19 at time 3, and 10 at time 4). See Table 1 for more detailed participant information.
Assessments
IQ
Standard scores in verbal, nonverbal, and overall ability (referred to as verbal IQ [VIQ], performance IQ [PIQ], and full-scale IQ [FSIQ], respectively, despite differences in terminology across the tests) were assessed in the participants with ASD and the participants with typical development at each of the four time points. At time 1, participants completed the Differential Abilities Scales [52], Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (third edition) [53], or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (third edition) [54] (depending on age and verbal ability). At time 2, participants completed the four subtests of the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI) [55]; at time 3, participants completed the two-subtest version of the WASI; and at time 4, participants completed the WASI if they had a previous IQ test, or participants completed the Differential Abilities Scales [52], Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (third edition) [53], or Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (third edition) [54] if they had not had a previous IQ test.
Imaging protocol
Participants were recruited and scanned at the University of Utah. Image processing and analyses were conducted at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. A total of 346 scans were obtained from the participants (221 ASD scans and 125 TDC scans). Magnetic resonance images were acquired on a Siemens Trio 3.0 Tesla Scanner (Siemens, Munich, Germany) at the University of Utah. At all four time points, the DTI acquisition used a product single-shot, spin-echo, echo planar imaging (EPI) pulse sequence with diffusion weighting, performed with bipolar gradients with dual-echo refocusing to reduce eddy currents [56]. Parallel imaging with a geometric reduction factor of two was used to reduce image distortions from magnetic field inhomogeneities. For each slice, we obtained diffusion-weighted images in 12 non-collinear diffusion encoding directions with a b = 1,000 s/mm2 and a single b = 0 image. Sixty contiguous axial slices 2.5 mm thick were acquired over the cerebrum and cerebellum (matrix = 128 × 128; field of view [FOV] = 256 mm; resolution = 2 × 2 × 2.5 mm; repetition time [TR] = 7,000 ms; echo time [TE] = 84 ms at time 1 and 91 ms at times 2, 3, and 4; and pixel bandwidth = 1,346 Hz). The acquisition was repeated four times (52 volumes total) in 6.5 min. Thirty-two young participants with ASD received sedation for scanning at time 1, and fifteen of these same participants received sedation again at time 2. No sedation was used at times 3 or 4. Sedation, using a combination of remifentanil and propofol, followed a strict clinical protocol approved by the institutional review board by the University of Utah and performed by an onsite faculty anesthesiologist. No complications occurred. In several cases, rehearsal was used to practice lying in the scanner.
Between times 1 and 2, the scanner hardware (primarily the head coil: an 8-channel receive-only array coil at time 1 and a 12-channel receive-only array coil at times 2, 3, and 4) and software were upgraded (which accounted for the TE change described above). To account for systematic differences in the imaging measurements at time 1 versus times 2, 3, and 4 due to this upgrade, a linear regression ‘head coil’ variable (which also accommodated for pulse sequence differences) was created and included in all statistical models. To further ascertain that the present longitudinal results were not solely due to noise from this upgrade, all patterns of results were tested by running time 1 analyses only and then by dropping time 1 and using only times 2, 3, and 4 data (see Additional file 1, Tables S1 and S2).
DTI image analysis
Diffusion-weighted images (DWI) were corrected for distortion, translation, and rotation from eddy currents and bulk head motion using an affine registration tools implemented in the fMRIB FSL software library [57]. To exclude regions of extreme intensities prior to processing, a mask was applied that excluded the upper 1% of the apparent diffusion coefficient, lower 1% of the average diffusion-weighted map, and lower 1% of the average b0 maps. The gradient orientation was corrected for rotation [58]. The DWI were smoothed in the axial plane using a 2D Gaussian kernel with 1.88-mm full width at half maximum (FWHM). Brain images were then skull-stripped. Next, the tensors were fit using a robust estimation algorithm (RESTORE) [59], implemented in Camino [60]. RESTORE has been shown to substantially reduce the effects of noise and physiologic artifacts from DTI maps by removing outliers in DWI measurements [61].
The eigenvalue maps (λ1, λ2, λ3) were computed from the estimated diffusion tensors. Maps of the MD (average of the eigenvalues), FA (the normalized standard deviation of the eigenvalues reflecting the relative degree of diffusion anisotropy), AD (λ1 the largest eigenvalue), and RD (λ3 diffusion perpendicular to the major eigenvector) were calculated. The units for MD, RD, and AD were square millimeter per second, scaled 10−3. Quality control checks were manually performed on the DTI images, looking for instances of slice intensity banding, FA hyperintensities, frontal lobe distortions, or blurring that could affect the analyses. Additionally, the white matter masks were checked to make sure that they were adequately covering the white matter in each brain scan. After quality control checks on the data, all but six ASD scans (98.6%) met the quality control standards.
To account for the effects of potential group differences in head motion during scanning, we computed the total motion index (TMI) for each participant as described in [62,63]. The TMI suggested that head motion did not differ between groups (P = 0.84) nor across ages (P = 0.48), but TMI was used as a covariate in all analyses to ascertain that group differences were not due to head motion. Additionally, to account for possible physiological noise, we computed the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) for each region of the corpus callosum in each scan by taking the mean signal from all the volumes of the scan (b0 and the diffusion weighted volumes), dividing the signal by the noise in a voxel (noise estimated using the residual method described in the appendix of [61]), and then calculating the average SNR within each corpus callosum region of each scan. SNR of each region was used as a covariate in all analyses of that particular region.
A population-specific template was estimated iteratively, aligning all the subjects using affine and diffeomorphic diffusion tensor registration implemented in DTI-TK [64]. To segment the corpus callosum and its subregions (that is, genu, body, and splenium), the JHU ICBM-DTI-81 template FA [65] was registered to our population FA template using a diffeomorphic spatial normalization tool, ANTS [66], and the subregion labels were transferred to our template using nearest neighbor interpolation. Using a method similar to that described by Faria and colleagues [67], these regions of interest (ROIs) were then transferred to the native space of each participant using the inverse of the corresponding spatial transformations estimated during the population normalization. Median FA, MD, RD, and AD were calculated for each ROI. The median was selected as our measure of central tendency rather than the mean to protect against sensitivity to voxels with extreme values. To prevent partial volume effects, we generated histograms of the MD values for each scan and masked out any voxels that had MD two standard deviations above the mean for each scan. Then, corpus callosum white matter was further refined by masking out voxels with FA less than two standard deviations below the mean for each corpus callosum mask.
Statistical analyses
To assess the reliability of the longitudinal DTI measurements, an intraclass correlation coefficient (ICC) was calculated for each statistical model by taking the within-individual variability in the mixed-effects model and dividing it by the sum of within and between variability [68]. Because development of the corpus callosum may not be fully captured by parametric methods, we took a semi-parametric approach that used penalized smoothing splines to examine age-related changes in DTI metrics. These are generalized additive mixed models [69-72] that also model repeated measurements per subject, are specially designed for our cohort-sequential sampling plan [73,74], and have been found to have empirical validity in different applications. These smoothing splines often bend to data more effectively than polynomials (while being penalized for each bend to prevent over-fitting), do not miss important nonlinear growth spurts or declines, and prevent fitted curve bias at the extremes of the data range. We used these smooth curves to examine the effects of age and diagnosis on the tensor coefficients, after taking into account head motion (TMI), head coil, and SNR. A centered FSIQ variable was initially included as a possible covariate, but it was in no case found to improve the fit significantly and therefore dropped. Finally, a number of more traditional general linear mixed-effects models were conducted to confirm the penalized spline models. Specifically, we divided the sample into three age groups (less than 10Â years of age, 10 to 20Â years of age, and more than 20Â years of age) to examine the main effects of diagnosis and age on DTI metrics as well as age-by-group interactions within each of these age brackets.
Results
Longitudinal data reliability and consistency
To examine the reliability of the present DTI data, we calculated ICCs for each DTI measurement of each subregion (see Table 2). The average ICC was 83%, demonstrating good reliability (although AD of the body of the corpus callosum had much lower ICC than the rest of the measurements). Furthermore, to ascertain that our typically developing data was representative of the population at large, we compared this data to previous typically developing DTI studies with similar age ranges (that is, [42,43]) and found similar means, ranges, and growth curves for FA despite differences in data acquisition and analysis procedures. Therefore, these data demonstrated good within-individual consistency and consistency with previous reports.
Longitudinal corpus callosum trajectory models
We tested for case-control differences in the tensor coefficients (FA, MD, RD, AD) and their longitudinal trajectories. FA measurements obtained from each of the three subregions of the corpus callosum (that is, the genu, body, and splenium) can be viewed in Figure 2. A figure of the cortical projections of each subregion of the corpus callosum can be seen in Additional file 2, which shows that the genu primarily contains projections to prefrontal cortices, the body contains projections premotor and supplementary motor frontal cortices to the primary somatosensory cortices, and the splenium contains projections to the parietal, occipital, and temporal lobes.
A generalized additive mixed-effects model analysis suggested decreased mean FA values in individuals with ASD in each of the corpus callosum subregions (see Table 2). These significant group differences in mean FA values were accompanied by a significant term for the age spline curve. In the genu and body, there were significant age-spline-by-group interactions, suggesting that the developmental trajectories of FA in those regions were different in the ASD group compared to the typically developing group. Follow-up linear mixed-effects models in different age groups (less than 10 years, 10 to 20 years, and over 20 years) revealed significant age-by-group interactions for FA in all subregions for individuals under 10 years of age, but there were no significant interactions for either of the older groups (Table 3).
Plots of MD, RD, and AD measurements obtained from the corpus callosum subregions are provided in Figure 3. Table 2 demonstrates significant group differences only in MD, RD, and AD of the splenium. However, it should be noted that even though splenium AD had a statistically significant group difference, it corresponded to a very small effect size (that is, on a 0.35% increase in AD in ASD), which means that it might not be a meaningful difference. The only significant age-by-group interaction was found in MD of the splenium. As in the FA follow-up results, linear mixed-effects models suggested that the majority of interaction effects occurred in the youngest age group (that is, participants less than 10 years of age).
Differential analyses
Follow-up analyses examined these same models under four separate conditions: 1) time 1 only (using 8-channel head coil), 2) times 2 to 4 (using the 12-channel head coil), 3) without participants who were on centrally active medications, and 4) without participants who were sedated during scanning. The figures and tables of results for these differential analyses can be seen in the supplementary materials (Additional file 1). Without individuals who were medicated, the pattern of results was virtually identical. Similarly, when only time 1 was analyzed, similarly sized main effects and interactions were present (although some were only marginally significant due to the loss of three fourths of the data). However, without the individuals who were sedated during scanning (mostly younger participants) and without the data from time 1 (when most of the younger children’s data was collected), significant interaction effects were no longer present. Therefore, under conditions in which younger children with ASD were not as represented in the model, there were no differences in the developmental trajectories between the ASD group and the typically developing group. Nevertheless, because many of the younger children’s scans are confounded with sedation effects, the present study cannot definitively disentangle atypical early development of the corpus callosum from sedation effects in these models.
Discussion
The present study used a cohort sequential design and generalized additive spline models to examine age-related trajectories of white matter microstructure of the corpus callosum in ASD compared to typical development. We aimed to longitudinally investigate development of corpus callosum microstructure in ASD from 3Â years of age through the final stages of corpus callosum development in young adulthood. Even after controlling for known confounds in the imaging literature such as head motion and SNR, the longitudinal results yielded significant group differences in the microstructure of corpus callosum across the entire period. FA appeared to be atypically increased during early childhood. Its developmental trajectory during the first 10Â years of life was in the opposite direction of typical development; FA decreased during childhood in the ASD group but increased as expected in TDC children. The ASD and TDC FA curves crossed during childhood, leading to a sustained decrease in FA in the ASD group relative to TDC during adolescence and young adulthood. Developmental abnormalities were found in all three subdivisions of the corpus callosum examined, predominantly affecting FA in the genu and body and MD in the splenium. These overall observations are bolstered by relative individual consistency of the longitudinal DTI measurements across the study and the consistency of these results with other investigations.
Developmental differences in microstructure of corpus callosum
The present study demonstrated significant group differences in the developmental trajectory of the microstructure of the genu and body (FA). This atypical developmental trajectory in ASD was characterized by atypically high FA values in early childhood that decreased with age, crossed the TDC curve, and plateaued below the FA values of the typically developing group. Follow-up analyses conducted within particular age brackets reiterated that the developmental slope of FA was atypical only in the youngest ASD cohort (less than 10Â years of age). This result is consistent with the only previous longitudinal investigation of corpus callosum microstructure in ASD [44], which examined microstructure in infants (6 to 24Â months of age) and found atypical developmental trajectories of FA in the corpus callosum of the group that went on to develop ASD. Our sample did not overlap in age with the Wolff et al. study [44], but our results converge to suggest higher corpus callosum FA in early childhood in ASD that may decrease with age. Cross-sectional studies have also found increased FA in young children with autism [32,35,75,76]. Our result of crossing of the ASD and TDC corpus callosum FA growth curves at about 7Â years of age is consistent with age-related findings in a cross-sectional study that suggested crossing between 7 and 8Â years of age [76]. Intersecting of the corpus callosum FA growth curve during late childhood is consistent with cross-sectional studies at this age that have found no group differences [77,78].
We also found atypical longitudinal development of corpus callosum microstructure during adolescence and young adulthood. FA of the genu and body was persistently decreased; rate of change was similar to the TDC group. Cross-sectional studies of ASD have consistently found decreased FA in adolescents and adults with ASD [27-31,79-85]. The longitudinal trajectories depicted in our models of corpus callosum FA demonstrate that the inconsistencies that have been noted in the corpus callosum DTI in ASD literature are likely due to the different age ranges of the different studies. The present group differences are also consistent with functional connectivity findings of decreased interhemispheric connectivity in ASD [2,17-19]. It is possible that the present findings of decreased structural connectivity of the corpus callosum in ASD may correspond to these functional differences, although a direct comparison will be needed to confirm this relation. Recent evidence suggests that the relationship between structural connectivity indexed by DTI and resting state functional connectivity across the lifespan is complex [86].
In addition to differences in FA, the only corpus callosum region that the ASD group exhibited significantly increased MD, RD, and AD was in the splenium, although the effect size of the splenium AD was much smaller than the other effects. Further, there was a significant group difference in the developmental trajectory of splenium MD, but this should be interpreted with caution because our follow-up analyses did not converge to show that this was a significant effect in any of our age brackets. Although DTI measures do not specify the type of white matter microstructural changes that may be present (for example, myelination, axonal size and packing, or glial cellularity), the present results (decreased FA the splenium in conjunction with increased MD and RD [but smaller effect sized for group differences in AD]) are consistent with abnormal myelination in ASD [24]. Speculatively, factors such as atypical early development or maintenance of myelin (occurring before birth or in the first few years of life) could be underlying these brain differences in ASD. These mechanisms may be better elucidated in future research through animal and human postmortem myelination studies or by combining in vivo DTI with more specific imaging measures of myelination, including multicomponent relaxometry and magnetization transfer [24]. Although MD and RD of the genu did not exhibit significant group differences, it should be noted that these non-significant effects were in the same direction as the splenium effects. Therefore, it may be reasonable to assume that the underlying mechanisms may be similar across the corpus callosum.
It is further important to note that we observed great variability in the corpus callosum DTI values of the ASD group. As can be seen in the individual data points presented in Figure 2, there were a number of individuals with ASD who had typical corpus callosum values and a subset of individuals with ASD who demonstrated atypically low FA, high MD, and high RD. In the time 1 analyses of this data, Alexander et al. noted that group differences in the corpus callosum DTI metrics appeared to be driven by a minority of ASD participants [27]. The present longitudinal results extend upon those findings to show that the heterogeneity in the ASD group remained and that this subset of participants continued to show atypical DTI values across the 9-year period of this study. Future studies will investigate behavioral and genetic contributions to the variation in FA/MD in the participants with extreme corpus callosum values.
The role of the corpus callosum in autism
Corpus callosum group differences have been the most consistently reported finding in the DTI in ASD literature [25]. Additionally, the corpus callosum repeatedly has been shown to be smaller in ASD [15,16], and functional imaging studies have demonstrated correlations between functional connectivity measures and the size of relevant regions of the corpus callosum [2,87-89]. The present longitudinal results reiterate corpus callosum pathology in ASD, and while white matter outside of the corpus callosum has been found to be affected in ASD, the corpus callosum should be given special consideration because of its size and of its participation in more global brain development. Specifically, the corpus callosum is the largest white matter tract in the brain, and it is known to reciprocally affect cortical development (see [46] for a review), which means that the corpus callosum could be intimately involved in the development of other structural brain differences that are observed in ASD. Indeed, atypical development of cortical thickness has been longitudinally observed in this sample [90], and future studies are critically needed to examine if corpus callosum atypicalities precede or co-occur with cortical thickness atypicalities in ASD.
Although there is converging evidence across multiple lines of research that suggest that the corpus callosum is commonly affected in ASD, the critical question of how corpus callosum atypicalities relate to the core behavioral symptoms of ASD remains. The similarity in some of the cognitive and symptom profiles between individuals with callosal agenesis and individuals with ASD is intriguing [91-93]. However, neither the literature nor the present study has established a one-to-one link between core ASD symptomatology and the corpus callosum. Perhaps, this is driven by the diversity of cognitive functions related to the corpus callosum [6], the development of compensatory mechanisms to accommodate corpus callosum atypicalities [94], the heterogeneity of symptom profiles within the ASD diagnosis, or the way in which corpus callosum development may be affecting or affected by cortical development [46]. Future research is needed to investigate how variation in specific neurocognitive functions may relate to variation in corpus callosum microstructure. Longitudinal studies provide the opportunity to discern causal pathways between alterations of brain structure, microstructure, and function and cognitive and behavioral impairments. Elucidation of causal pathways and mediating mechanisms, and development of predictive models, will pave the way for the formation of new biologically informed interventions and their targeted use in specific ASD subgroups.
Limitations
There are several potential limitations associated with this present study. First, a limitation of the cohort sequential design is the use of a multiple cohorts across ages, which may make this design less sensitive to age-specific changes during narrow windows of development. However, the cohort sequential design allowed us to more rapidly answer this key longitudinal question in autism than would have been possible with a longitudinal panel design, and the accelerated design also minimized potential cohort-related confounds [95]. A second possible limitation is that the DTI protocol was held as consistent as possible across all data collection in this longitudinal study, despite the fact that more optimal protocols (for example, more than 12 directions, isotropic spatial resolution) have been developed over the years. In addition, there were unforeseeable changes in the hardware (head coil) and subsequent product DTI pulse sequence (TE) between time 1 and time 2 that resulted in a systematic bias in DTI measures between these two time points. As a result, a covariate for scanner was included in the age-trajectory models, which appears to have removed the bias associated with the imaging time point. The corrected measures were highly consistent across all time points, and the models were very similar without individuals with medication and when only time 1 data were included. However, the interaction effects were not sustained when eliminating participants under sedation and only times 2 through 4 data (both strategies that eliminated our youngest participants), which means that we cannot definitively disambiguate between the effects of age or sedation on our group differences in developmental trajectories. Sedation during pediatric neuroimaging has been shown to be safe and effective [96], and while there is evidence that there could be minimal effects of sedation on DTI metrics in temporal white matter [97], it is unclear whether this is due to the actual sedation or to the fact that the most severely impacted individuals with ASD were most likely to be sedated. Therefore, the effects of sedation on DTI are still unclear and will need to be elucidated in future research. Our study focused on verbal males to decrease heterogeneity in the ASD sample and increase statistical power. Additional studies of females and intellectually low-functioning individuals are needed to determine if the findings of the present study generalize to these groups. Finally, given the heterogeneity present even in verbal males with autism, replication of our findings in an independent sample is needed.
Conclusions
The present study used DTI methods to longitudinally examine the white matter microstructure of the corpus callosum in individuals with ASD compared to typically developing controls (ages 3 to 41Â years). The results suggest that corpus callosum microstructure (especially in the anterior corpus callosum) develops atypically in autism before the age of 10Â years and that this early atypical development becomes a persistent group difference in later childhood, adolescence, and adulthood. Because the corpus callosum is the largest white matter tract in the brain, corpus callosum atypicalities likely have lasting effects on many aspects of cognition and brain synchronization in ASD.
Abbreviations
- AD:
-
axial diffusivity
- ADOS:
-
Autism Diagnostic Observation Scale
- ASD:
-
autism Spectrum Disorder
- DTI:
-
diffusion tensor imaging
- DWI:
-
diffusion-weighted images
- EPI:
-
echo planar imaging
- FA:
-
fractional anisotropy
- FOV:
-
field of view
- FSIQ:
-
full-scale IQ
- FWHM:
-
full width at half maximum
- ICC:
-
intraclass correlation coefficient
- MD:
-
mean diffusivity
- PIQ:
-
performance IQ
- RD:
-
radial diffusivity
- RESTORE:
-
robust estimation algorithm
- ROI:
-
region of interest
- SNR:
-
signal-to-noise ratio
- TE:
-
echo time
- TMI:
-
total motion index
- TR:
-
repetition time
- VIQ:
-
verbal IQ
- WASI:
-
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence
References
Belmonte MK, Allen G, Beckel-Mitchener A, Boulanger LM, Carper RA, Webb SJ. Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2004;24:9228–31.
Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Kana RK, Minshew NJ. Functional and anatomical cortical underconnectivity in autism: evidence from an FMRI study of an executive function task and corpus callosum morphometry. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17:951–61.
Kana RK, Keller TA, Cherkassky VL, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Sentence comprehension in autism: thinking in pictures with decreased functional connectivity. Brain J Neurol. 2006;129(Pt 9):2484–93.
Kitzbichler MG, Khan S, Ganesan S, Vangel MG, Herbert MR, Hämäläinen MS, et al. Altered development and multifaceted band-specific abnormalities of resting state networks in autism. Biol Psychiatry 2014
Pajevic S, Basser PJ, Fields RD. Role of myelin plasticity in oscillations and synchrony of neuronal activity. Neuroscience. 2014;276:135–47.
Paul LK, Brown WS, Adolphs R, Tyszka JM, Richards LJ, Mukherjee P, et al. Agenesis of the corpus callosum: genetic, developmental and functional aspects of connectivity. Nat Rev. 2007;8:287–99.
Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc JINS. 1997;3:303–16.
Minshew NJ, Sweeney J, Luna B. Autism as a selective disorder of complex information processing and underdevelopment of neocortical systems. Mol Psychiatry. 2002;7 Suppl 2:S14–5.
Williams DL, Goldstein G, Minshew NJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in children with autism: further evidence for disordered complex information-processing. Child Neuropsychol J Norm Abnorm Dev Child Adolesc. 2006;12:279–98.
Courchesne E, Press GA, Yeung-Courchesne R. Parietal lobe abnormalities detected with MR in patients with infantile autism. AJR American J Roentgenol. 1993;160:387–93.
Belmonte M, Egaas B, Townsend J, Courchesne E. NMR intensity of corpus callosum differs with age but not with diagnosis of autism. Neuroreport. 1995;6:1253–6.
Egaas B, Courchesne E, Saitoh O. Reduced size of corpus callosum in autism. Arch Neurol. 1995;52:794–801.
Piven J, Bailey J, Ranson BJ, Arndt S. An MRI study of the corpus callosum in autism. Am J Psychiatry. 1997;154:1051–6.
Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Keshavan MS. Corpus callosum size in autism. Neurology. 2000;55:1033–6.
Prigge MB, Lange N, Bigler ED, Merkley TL, Neeley ES, Abildskov TJ, et al. Corpus callosum area in children and adults with autism. Res Autism Spectr Disord. 2013;7:221–34.
Frazier TW, Hardan AY. A meta-analysis of the corpus callosum in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66:935–41.
Di Martino A, Ross K, Uddin LQ, Sklar AB, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. Functional brain correlates of social and nonsocial processes in autism spectrum disorders: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;65:63–74.
Anderson JS, Druzgal TJ, Froehlich A, DuBray MB, Lange N, Alexander AL, et al. Decreased interhemispheric functional connectivity in autism. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21:1134–46.
Dinstein I, Pierce K, Eyler L, Solso S, Malach R, Behrmann M, et al. Disrupted neural synchronization in toddlers with autism. Neuron. 2011;70:1218–25.
Lamantia AS, Rakic P. Cytological and quantitative characteristics of four cerebral commissures in the rhesus monkey. J Comp Neurol. 1990;291:520–37.
Basser PJ, Mattiello J, LeBihan D. MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy and imaging. Biophys J. 1994;66:259–67.
Song SK, Sun SW, Ramsbottom MJ, Chang C, Russell J, Cross AH. Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but unchanged axial) diffusion of water. NeuroImage. 2002;17:1429–36.
Song SK, Yoshino J, Le TQ, Lin SJ, Sun SW, Cross AH, et al. Demyelination increases radial diffusivity in corpus callosum of mouse brain. NeuroImage. 2005;26:132–40.
Alexander AL, Hurley SA, Samsonov AA, Adluru N, Hosseinbor AP, Mossahebi P, et al. Characterization of cerebral white matter properties using quantitative magnetic resonance imaging stains. Brain Connect. 2011;1:423–46.
Travers BG, Adluru N, Ennis C, Tromp DPM, Destiche D, Doran S, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging in autism spectrum disorder: a review. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2012;5:289–313.
Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Field AS. Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain. Neurother J Am Soc Exp Neurother. 2007;4:316–29.
Alexander AL, Lee JE, Lazar M, Boudos R, DuBray MB, Oakes TR, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of the corpus callosum in autism. NeuroImage. 2007;34:61–73.
Ameis SH, Fan J, Rockel C, Voineskos AN, Lobaugh NJ, Soorya L, et al. Impaired structural connectivity of socio-emotional circuits in autism spectrum disorders: a diffusion tensor imaging study. PLoS One. 2011;6:e28044.
Jeong JW, Kumar AK, Sundaram SK, Chugani HT, Chugani DC. Sharp curvature of frontal lobe white matter pathways in children with autism spectrum disorders: tract-based morphometry analysis. AJNR American J Neuroradiol. 2011;32:1600–6.
Shukla DK, Keehn B, Lincoln AJ, Muller RA. White matter compromise of callosal and subcortical fiber tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2010;49:1269–78.
Shukla DK, Keehn B, Smylie DM, Muller RA. Microstructural abnormalities of short-distance white matter tracts in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1378–82.
Weinstein M, Ben-Sira L, Levy Y, Zachor DA, Ben Itzhak E, Artzi M, et al. Abnormal white matter integrity in young children with autism. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32:534–43.
Harsan LA, Poulet P, Guignard B, Steibel J, Parizel N, de Sousa PL, et al. Brain dysmyelination and recovery assessment by noninvasive in vivo diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci Res. 2006;83:392–402.
Tyszka JM, Readhead C, Bearer EL, Pautler RG, Jacobs RE. Statistical diffusion tensor histology reveals regional dysmyelination effects in the shiverer mouse mutant. NeuroImage. 2006;29:1058–65.
Ben Bashat D, Kronfeld-Duenias V, Zachor DA, Ekstein PM, Hendler T, Tarrasch R, et al. Accelerated maturation of white matter in young children with autism: a high b value DWI study. NeuroImage. 2007;37:40–7.
Cheng Y, Chou KH, Chen IY, Fan YT, Decety J, Lin CP. Atypical development of white matter microstructure in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage. 2010;50:873–82.
Cheung C, Chua SE, Cheung V, Khong PL, Tai KS, Wong TK, et al. White matter fractional anisotrophy differences and correlates of diagnostic symptoms in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009;50:1102–12.
Ashtari M, Cervellione KL, Hasan KM, Wu J, McIlree C, Kester H, et al. White matter development during late adolescence in healthy males: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. NeuroImage. 2007;35:501–10.
Barnea-Goraly N, Menon V, Eckert M, Tamm L, Bammer R, Karchemskiy A, et al. White matter development during childhood and adolescence: a cross-sectional diffusion tensor imaging study. Cereb Cortex. 2005;15:1848–54.
Giorgio A, Watkins KE, Douaud G, James AC, James S, De Stefano N, et al. Changes in white matter microstructure during adolescence. NeuroImage. 2008;39:52–61.
Giorgio A, Santelli L, Tomassini V, Bosnell R, Smith S, De Stefano N, et al. Age-related changes in grey and white matter structure throughout adulthood. NeuroImage. 2010;51:943–51.
Lebel C, Walker L, Leemans A, Phillips L, Beaulieu C. Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood. NeuroImage. 2008;40:1044–55.
Lebel C, Beaulieu C. Longitudinal development of human brain wiring continues from childhood into adulthood. J Neurosci Off J Soc Neurosci. 2011;31:10937–47.
Wolff JJ, Gu H, Gerig G, Elison JT, Styner M, Gouttard S, et al. Differences in white matter fiber tract development present from 6 to 24 months in infants with autism. Am J Psychiatry 2012
Innocenti GM. Dynamic interactions between the cerebral hemispheres. Exp Brain Res Exp Hirnforsch Expérimentation Cérébrale. 2009;192:417–23.
Pietrasanta M, Restani L, Caleo M. The corpus callosum and the visual cortex: plasticity is a game for two. Neural Plast. 2012;2012:838672.
Nesselroade JR, Baltes PB. Longitudinal research in the study of behavior and development. Academic Press; 1979
Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A. Autism diagnostic interview-revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24:659–85.
Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, et al. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30:205–23.
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic criteria from DSM-IV-TR. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2000.
Jeste SS, Geschwind DH. Disentangling the heterogeneity of autism spectrum disorder through genetic findings. Nat Rev Neurol. 2014;10:74–81.
Elliott C. The nature and structure of children’s abilities: evidence from the differential ability scales. J Psychoeduc Assess. 1990;8:376–90.
Wechsler D. The Wechsler intelligence scale for children. 3rd ed. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation; 1991.
Wechsler D. Wechsler adult iintelligence scale-III (WAIS-III). San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 1997.
Wechsler D. Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. New York: The Psychological Corporation: Harcourt Brace & Company; 1999.
Reese TG, Heid O, Weisskoff RM, Wedeen VJ. Reduction of eddy-current-induced distortion in diffusion MRI using a twice-refocused spin echo. Magn Reson Med Off J Soc Magn Reson Med Soc Magn Reson Med. 2003;49:177–82.
Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TE, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. NeuroImage. 2012;62:782–90.
Leemans A, Jones DK. The B-matrix must be rotated when correcting for subject motion in DTI data. Magn Reson Med Off J Soc Magn Reson Med Soc Magn Reson Med. 2009;61:1336–49.
Chang LC, Jones DK, Pierpaoli C. RESTORE: robust estimation of tensors by outlier rejection. Magn Reson Med Off J Soc Magn Reson Med Soc Magn Reson Med. 2005;53:1088–95.
Cook PA, Bai Y, Nedjati-Gilanio S, Seunarine KK, Hall MG, Parker GJ, Alexander DC: Camino: open-source diffusion-MRI reconstruction and processing. Volume Seattle, WA; 2006:2759.
Walker L, Chang LC, Koay CG, Sharma N, Cohen L, Verma R, et al. Effects of physiological noise in population analysis of diffusion tensor MRI data. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1168–77.
Benner T, van der Kouwe AJW, Sorensen AG. Diffusion imaging with prospective motion correction and reacquisition. Magn Reson Med Off J Soc Magn Reson Med Soc Magn Reson Med. 2011;66:154–67.
Yendiki A, Koldewyn K, Kakunoori S, Kanwisher N, Fischl B. Spurious group differences due to head motion in a diffusion MRI study. NeuroImage. 2013;88C:79–90.
Zhang H, Yushkevich PA, Alexander DC, Gee JC. Deformable registration of diffusion tensor MR images with explicit orientation optimization. Med Image Anal. 2006;10:764–85.
Mori S, Wakana S, Nagae-Poetscher L, Van Zijl PC. MRI atlas of human white matter. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 2005.
Avants B, Gee JC. Geodesic estimation for large deformation anatomical shape averaging and interpolation. NeuroImage. 2004;23 Suppl 1:S139–50.
Faria AV, Hoon A, Stashinko E, Li X, Jiang H, Mashayekh A, et al. Quantitative analysis of brain pathology based on MRI and brain atlases - applications for cerebral palsy. NeuroImage. 2011;54:1854–61.
Walker L, Curry M, Nayak A, Lange N, Pierpaoli C. A framework for the analysis of phantom data in multicenter diffusion tensor imaging studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2012
Hastie T. Generalized additive models. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 1999.
Wood SN. Stable and efficient multiple smoothing parameter estimation for generalized additive models. J Am Stat Assoc. 2004;99
Wood SN. Generalized additive models: an introduction with R. Boca Raton, FL: Chapman & Hall/CRC; 2006 [Texts in Statistical Science].
Wood S, Scheipl F. gamm4: generalized additive mixed models using Mgcv and lme4. 2013.
Alexander-Bloch AF, Reiss PT, Rapoport J, McAdams H, Giedd JN, Bullmore ET, et al. Abnormal cortical growth in schizophrenia targets normative modules of synchronized development. Biol Psychiatry. 2014;76:438–46.
Harezlak J, Ryan LM, Giedd JN, Lange N. Individual and population penalized regression splines for accelerated longitudinal designs. Biometrics. 2005;61:1037–48.
Xiao Z, Qiu T, Ke X, Xiao X, Xiao T, Liang F, et al. Autism spectrum disorder as early neurodevelopmental disorder: evidence from the brain imaging abnormalities in 2-3 years old toddlers. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:1633–40.
Billeci L, Calderoni S, Tosetti M, Catani M, Muratori F. White matter connectivity in children with autism spectrum disorders: a tract-based spatial statistics study. BMC Neurol. 2012;12:148.
Koldewyn K, Yendiki A, Weigelt S, Gweon H, Julian J, Richardson H, et al. Differences in the right inferior longitudinal fasciculus but no general disruption of white matter tracts in children with autism spectrum disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2014;111:1981–6.
Hanaie R, Mohri I, Kagitani-Shimono K, Tachibana M, Matsuzaki J, Watanabe Y, et al. Abnormal corpus callosum connectivity, socio-communicative deficits, and motor deficits in children with autism spectrum disorder: a diffusion tensor imaging study. J Autism Dev Disord. 2014;44:2209–20.
Barnea-Goraly N, Kwon H, Menon V, Eliez S, Lotspeich L, Reiss AL. White matter structure in autism: preliminary evidence from diffusion tensor imaging. Biol Psychiatry. 2004;55:323–6.
Barnea-Goraly N, Lotspeich LJ, Reiss AL. Similar white matter aberrations in children with autism and their unaffected siblings: a diffusion tensor imaging study using tract-based spatial statistics. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67:1052–60.
Brito AR, Vasconcelos MM, Domingues RC, da Cruz Hygino LC, Lde Rodrigues S, Gasparetto EL, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging findings in school-aged autistic children. J Neuroimaging Off J Am Soc Neuroimaging. 2009;19:337–43.
Cheon KA, Kim YS, Oh SH, Park SY, Yoon HW, Herrington J, et al. Involvement of the anterior thalamic radiation in boys with high functioning autism spectrum disorders: a Diffusion Tensor Imaging study. Brain Res. 2011;1417:77–86.
Jou RJ, Jackowski AP, Papademetris X, Rajeevan N, Staib LH, Volkmar FR. Diffusion tensor imaging in autism spectrum disorders: preliminary evidence of abnormal neural connectivity. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2011;45:153–62.
Kumar A, Sundaram SK, Sivaswamy L, Behen ME, Makki MI, Ager J, et al. Alterations in frontal lobe tracts and corpus callosum in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Cereb Cortex. 2010;20:2103–13.
Noriuchi M, Kikuchi Y, Yoshiura T, Kira R, Shigeto H, Hara T, et al. Altered white matter fractional anisotropy and social impairment in children with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Res. 2010;1362:141–9.
Betzel RF, Byrge L, He Y, Goñi J, Zuo X-N, Sporns O. Changes in structural and functional connectivity among resting-state networks across the human lifespan. NeuroImage. 2014;102P2:345–57.
Mason RA, Williams DL, Kana RK, Minshew N, Just MA. Theory of mind disruption and recruitment of the right hemisphere during narrative comprehension in autism. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:269–80.
Damarla SR, Keller TA, Kana RK, Cherkassky VL, Williams DL, Minshew NJ, et al. Cortical underconnectivity coupled with preserved visuospatial cognition in autism: evidence from an fMRI study of an embedded figures task. Autism Res Off J Int Soc Autism Res. 2010;3:273–9.
Schipul SE, Williams DL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ, Just MA. Distinctive neural processes during learning in autism. Cereb Cortex. 2012;22:937–50.
Zielinski BA, Prigge MBD, Nielsen JA, Froehlich AL, Abildskov TJ, Anderson JS, et al. Longitudinal changes in cortical thickness in autism and typical development. Brain J Neurol. 2014;137:1799–812.
Booth R, Wallace GL, Happe F. Connectivity and the corpus callosum in autism spectrum conditions: insights from comparison of autism and callosal agenesis. Prog Brain Res. 2011;189:303–17.
Paul LK. Developmental malformation of the corpus callosum: a review of typical callosal development and examples of developmental disorders with callosal involvement. J Neurodev Disord. 2011;3:3–27.
Paul LK, Corsello C, Kennedy DP, Adolphs R. Agenesis of the corpus callosum and autism: a comprehensive comparison. Brain J Neurol. 2014;137(Pt 6):1813–29.
Hannay HJ, Dennis M, Kramer L, Blaser S, Fletcher JM. Partial agenesis of the corpus callosum in spina bifida meningomyelocele and potential compensatory mechanisms. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2009;31:180–94.
Willett JB, Singer JD, Martin NC. The design and analysis of longitudinal studies of development and psychopathology in context: statistical models and methodological recommendations. Dev Psychopathol. 1998;10:395–426.
Amundsen LB, Artru AA, Dager SR, Shaw DWW, Friedman S, Sparks B, et al. Propofol sedation for longitudinal pediatric neuroimaging research. J Neurosurg Anesthesiol. 2005;17:180–92.
Lee JE, Bigler ED, Alexander AL, Lazar M, DuBray MB, Chung MK, et al. Diffusion tensor imaging of white matter in the superior temporal gyrus and temporal stem in autism. Neurosci Lett. 2007;424:127–32.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health [RO1 MH080826 to JEL, ALA, NL, EDB; RO1 MH084795 to JEL, PTF, NL; K08 MH100609 to BAZ, and KO8 MH092697 to JSA], the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [T32 HD07489 to BGT, and P30 HD003352 to the Waisman Center], the Poelman Foundation [to EDB], the Primary Children’s Foundation [Early Career Development Award to BAZ], and the Hartwell Foundation [Postdoctoral Fellowship Award to BGT]. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institute of Mental Health, the National Institute of Child Health & Development, or the National Institutes of Health. We thank Zhan Xu, Anne M. Bartosic, Annahir Cariello, Celeste Knoles, Sam Doran, Danica Samsin, Kristine McLaughlin, and Abigail Freeman for their contributions to this project. We sincerely thank the children, adolescents, and adults with autism, the individuals with typical development, and all the families who participated in this study.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Additional information
Competing interests
All authors declare that they have no competing interests.
Authors’ contributions
BGT analyzed the data and drafted the manuscript. DPMT, NA, DD, and CE preprocessed the DTI data, provided quality control checks on the DTI data, and contributed to writing and revision of the manuscript. NL and PTF helped implement the general additive spline modeling and contributed to writing and revision of the manuscript. JA, ALF, and MBDP collected the data, provided quality control checks, and contributed to the writing and revision of the manuscript. JSA, BAZ, EDB, JEL, and ALA participated in the study design, coordination, conception, and writing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.
Additional files
Additional file 1:
Supplementary materials for differential analyses. This file contains five tables (Tables S1-S5) and eight figures (Figures S1-S8) that present the results of our differential analyses.
Additional file 2:
Projections of the genu, body, and splenium subsections of the corpus callosum. This picture file demonstrates the cortical projections of the genu (red), body (green), and splenium (blue) subregions of the corpus callosum in our study’s template space.
Rights and permissions
This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly credited. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.
About this article
Cite this article
Travers, B.G., Tromp, D.P.M., Adluru, N. et al. Atypical development of white matter microstructure of the corpus callosum in males with autism: a longitudinal investigation. Molecular Autism 6, 15 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0001-8
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-015-0001-8