Skip to main content

Altered frontal connectivity as a mechanism for executive function deficits in fragile X syndrome

Abstract

Background

Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the leading inherited monogenic cause of intellectual disability and autism spectrum disorder. Executive function (EF), necessary for adaptive goal-oriented behavior and dependent on frontal lobe function, is impaired in individuals with FXS. Yet, little is known how alterations in frontal lobe neural activity is related to EF deficits in FXS.

Methods

Sixty-one participants with FXS (54% males) and 71 age- and sex-matched typically-developing controls (TDC; 58% males) completed a five-minute resting state electroencephalography (EEG) protocol and a computerized battery of tests of EF, the Test of Attentional Performance for Children (KiTAP). Following source localization (minimum-norm estimate), we computed debiased weighted phase lag index (dWPLI), a phase connectivity value, for pairings between 18 nodes in frontal regions for gamma (30–55 Hz) and alpha (10.5–12.5 Hz) bands. Linear models were generated with fixed factors of group, sex, frequency, and connection. Relationships between frontal connectivity and EF variables also were examined.

Results

Individuals with FXS demonstrated increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity across all frontal regions and across hemispheres compared to TDC. After controlling for nonverbal IQ, increased error rates on EF tasks were associated with increased gamma band and reduced alpha band connectivity.

Limitations

Frontal connectivity findings are limited to intrinsic brain activity during rest and may not generalize to frontal connectivity during EF tasks or everyday function.

Conclusions

We report gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity within source-localized frontal brain regions in FXS compared to TDC during resting-state EEG. For the first time in FXS, we report significant associations between EF and altered frontal connectivity, with increased error rate relating to increased gamma band connectivity and reduced alpha band connectivity. These findings suggest increased phase connectivity within gamma band may impair EF performance, whereas greater alpha band connectivity may provide compensatory support for EF. Together, these findings provide important insight into neurophysiological mechanisms of EF deficits in FXS and provide novel targets for treatment development.

Background

Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the leading monogenic inherited form of intellectual disability (ID) and autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and is caused by a cysteine-guanine-guanine (CGG) trinucleotide repeat expansion in the fragile X messenger ribonucleoprotein 1 (FMR1) gene (> 200 repeats) located on the X chromosome [1, 2]. The repeat trinucleotide expansion in the FMR1 gene leads to a decrease or absence of fragile X messenger ribonucleoprotein (FMRP) expression, which is essential for brain development and cognitive function [2].

It is well documented that individuals with FXS exhibit impairments in executive functions (EF), an important set of cognitive functions involved in adaptive goal-oriented behavior, including subdomains of cognitive flexibility, working memory, response inhibition, and processing speed [3]. EF impairments are present to a lesser degree in females with FXS compared to males with FXS, as expected based on obligate mosaicism [4, 5]. However, even in the absence of general cognitive impairment in females with FXS, EF impairments are still observed [3]. Likewise, even though individuals with FXS are at a disproportionally higher risk of a comorbid attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis, EF impairments are present even in the absence of ADHD. EF impairments can cause significant distress to both the individual with FXS and their families and is a common reason for clinic visits [6,7,8]. Yet, our lack of understanding of its underlying physiology has stalled treatment development targeting EF for individuals with FXS despite its critical clinical significance.

EF historically has been associated with frontal lobe function with distinct domains of EF demonstrating a degree of regional specificity within the frontal lobe [9]. Although few structural or functional magnetic resonance imaging (MRI and fMRI, respectively) studies have been conducted in individuals with FXS (for review, see), differences in frontal lobe structure and function are present (even after controlling for differences in total brain volume) and related to impaired EF in this patient population. For example, individuals with FXS have shown reduced frontal lobe gray [10, 11] and white matter volume [10] as well as reduced activation of frontal regions during EF tasks of working memory and inhibitory control [12, 13].

MRI and fMRI studies in this population are inherently challenging since cardinal features of the disorder (e.g., anxiety, hyperactivity) prevent many full mutation males with FXS from participating, thus limiting the ability to generalize findings. Electroencephalography (EEG) offers a minimally invasive neuroimaging technique that can be more broadly applied in this population while still providing novel insights into the neurophysiological features of frontal lobe dysfunction [14,15,16]. It also can be more readily synchronously leveraged in preclinical studies using the mouse model of the disorder (for example, see [17]). Our previous EEG studies have demonstrated that individuals with FXS at rest have reduced alpha power, but increased gamma power, which together suggest cortical hyperexcitability [16, 18], a finding that has been replicated in in vivo slice physiology and mouse studies [17, 19,20,21]. Notably, sex differences in resting state alpha and gamma power differences have emerged in FXS, demonstrating that electrophysiological alterations are more pronounced in males with FXS [15, 22].

In addition, our group [16] has previously studied whole-brain connectivity in different frequency bands using EEG. Individuals with FXS demonstrated reduced inhibitory (alpha band) and enhanced excitatory (gamma band) connections across frontal, parietal, and occipital regions relative to age-matched controls. Similar findings of reduced alpha band connectivity in FXS, including within frontal regions, have been reported by other groups [14, 23]. Yet, likely in part due to its relatively small sample sizes, sex-specific connectivity patterns have not been shown in FXS. Since frontal oscillatory activity has established importance for optimal EF task performance [24,25,26], increased neural excitability in the frontal cortex may contribute to the cognitive alterations seen clinically in FXS. Of note, our group recently demonstrated a relationship between reduced phase locking to a chirp auditory stimulus and increased flexibility and distractibility errors [27], suggesting an important link between atypical neural dynamics and EF. Still, EEG studies in FXS largely have reported on small samples (n < 20) and have not been source-localized, and connectivity features have not been examined in relation to EF performance nor contrasted in male and female patients. Thus, examining phase connectivity within the frontal cortex and its relation to performance-based EF measures may provide novel insight into the functional relevance of altered alpha and gamma band oscillatory activity in FXS.

The present study compared intrinsic resting frontal phase connectivity in individuals with FXS and matched typically developing controls (TDC) and examined the relationship between frontal connectivity and performance-based measures of EF in FXS. We predicted that individuals with FXS would display increased connectivity of gamma oscillations but reduced connectivity of alpha oscillations in frontal cortex compared to TDC, and these findings would be particularly pronounced among males with FXS consistent with our resting power findings. We also predicted that frontal connectivity alterations would be associated with greater EF dysfunction in FXS, even after controlling for general cognitive functioning.

Methods

Participants

Sixty-one participants with a genetic diagnosis of FXS (mean age = 21.0, SD = 10.2; age range: 5.9–45.7; 28 females) and 71 age- and sex-matched controls (mean age = 22.2, SD = 10.7; age range: 5.9–48.2; 30 females) participated in study procedures (Table 1). Diagnosis of FXS was confirmed via Southern Blot and polymerase chain reaction (PCR) assays performed at Rush University in the laboratory of Dr. Elizabeth Berry-Kravis. Ten males with mosaicism (methylation or size) were included in the study and in all analyses unless otherwise indicated. FXS participants were excluded from the study if they were being treated for seizures within the past year or taking benzodiazepines, which are known to impact electrophysiological recordings. Several FXS participants were being treated with psychiatric medications at the time of testing, but were on stable dosing for at least six weeks. Twenty-one participants with FXS were receiving stimulants, 14 antipsychotics, and 28 antidepressants. Two controls were receiving stable dosing of SSRIs with no active psychiatric symptoms; removal of these participants from analysis did not result in any substantive changes in results and thus were included in final analyses.

Table 1 Participant demographic information

Participants completed the abbreviated Stanford Binet-5th edition (SB-5, [28]) to estimate general cognitive functioning. Standard scores were converted to deviation IQ scores, and scaled scores were converted to z-scores in order to reduce floor effects present for individuals with severe cognitive impairments and to better evaluate inter-individual variability [29]. All participants or their legal guardian, when appropriate, provided written informed consent and assent before participating. The study was approved by Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center institutional review board.

Data acquisition

In order to facilitate cooperation during EEG data collection, participants were seated comfortably while watching a silent video as done in previous studies [15, 16, 18, 30]. Five minutes of continuous resting EEG was collected by 128-channel EGI HydroCel Geodesic Sensor Net with a sampling rate at 1000 Hz. As previously described, data were preprocessed by filtering, visual inspection on 2 s epochs and channels, and ICA artifact removal (EEGLAB)[15, 18]. After preprocessing, an average of 125 epochs (SD: 20.8, range 44–187) for FXS and 131 (SD: 18.1, range 43–161) epochs for TDC of artifact-free data remained with no group differences (p = 0.10).

Neural connectivity

As a method recently validated in FXS [22], source-localized time series for each subject were constructed by the minimum norm estimate method in Brainstorm [31]. The reconstructed cortical model consisted of 15,002 vertices which were parcellated into 68 nodes via the Desikan–Killiany atlas [32]. Subsequent analysis steps were focused on eighteen nodes which were designated in frontal regions known contributions to EF [33,34,35] (Additional file 1).

Time series decomposition was performed via a series of Morlet wavelets where the frequencies included 10.5–12.5 Hz with a step size of 0.5 Hz representing the upper alpha band range and 30–55 Hz with a step size of 5 Hz representing the gamma band range. These bands were selected based on their relevance in previous FXS and EF studies [36,37,38,39,40,41,42]. Debiased weighted phase lag index (dWPLI), a measure that quantifies phase lead and lag relationship from a pair of signals [43], was chosen to study frequency-dependent spatial phase synchronization.

dWPLI was selected over other phase connectivity measures for its robustness against volume conduction in hypothesis-driven studies [44, 45]. The dWPLI estimates are negatively biased and has a theoretical range of [− 1, 1], such that zero and negative dWPLI values indicate lack of synchrony and one denotes consistent synchrony between two signals. Properties of the dWPLI measure, including the equation used for dWPLI, are discussed in supplementary materials (Additional file 2). With stationarity assumption from resting state EEG, dWPLI values were averaged over epochs per connection within each participant to characterize overall signal coherence, with an outlier criterion such that values greater than three standard deviations outside their individual mean were rejected.

Computerized testing of EF

Performance-based EF was estimated using the Test of Attentional Performance for Children (KiTAP) [46], a computerized measure of EF that is reproducible and clinically valid in individuals with FXS [47]. Participants completed four subtests: Alertness (processing speed), Distractibility (attention), Go/NoGo (response inhibition), and Flexibility (cognitive flexibility). Prior to each subtest, participants received verbal instructions and completed a practice task to ensure comprehension. Due to poor comprehension, five individuals with FXS did not complete Distractibility, five did not complete Go/NoGo, and eleven did not complete Flexibility tests. All controls completed subtests with the exception of one who did not complete Go/NoGo due to a technical issue.

A total of eight KiTAP variables were selected for analysis based on a priori hypotheses. Six KiTAP variables have established clinical validity and reproducibility in FXS [47]: Median response times for Alertness (1) and Flexibility (2); Standard deviation of response times for Alertness (3); and Number of errors for Distractibility (4), Go/NoGo (5), and Flexibility (6). An additional two variables, median response times for Distractibility (7) and Go/NoGo (8), were included based on previous studies documenting impaired speed and accuracy trade-off during tasks of cognitive control in neurodevelopmental disorders [3, 48].

Statistics

Linear mixed effect models (from R library lme4; [49]) were constructed to test for group differences for each connection region and frequency band. Connection regions were pre-defined within and across hemispheres. We grouped connectivity measures as: (1) left prefrontal, (2) right prefrontal, (3) left (posterior) frontal, and (4) right (posterior) frontal. Last, (5) Connections with identical cross-hemispheric nodes also were evaluated. Single region comparisons analyzed connections within that particular region. In each model, the response variable was dWPLI values transformed (Box-Cox 1-parameter) for residual normality. Each model started with all 4 fixed effects (group, sex, connection, and frequency) and a random effect of subject as an intercept. For fixed effect frequency, we used step sizes consistent with time series decomposition for both alpha and gamma frequency bands (alpha: 10.5–12.5 Hz, 0.5 Hz step; gamma: 30–55 Hz, 5 Hz step). The final model was based on a parsimonious principle on the fixed side. To account for differences in general cognitive functioning between groups which may impact connectivity findings, all models also were tested with nonverbal IQ included as a covariate. In FXS, nonverbal IQ is a better representation of overall cognitive functioning and controls for the potential confounding influence of language abilities on this overall score.

Additional models were examined within the FXS group only. First, to further examine whether males with FXS and females with FXS differ from each other as previously implicated by our work [15, 22], group was dropped from our model, but otherwise the model remained the same, including examining with and without nonverbal IQ as a covariate. Second, to determine whether participants using stimulants at the time of testing impacted findings, we added stimulant usage as an additional fixed effect within the model. For all models, maximum likelihood was used for parameter estimation and the Satterthwaite approximation was used for the degrees of freedom. P values were adjusted for multiple pair comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR).

Clinical correlations

To examine the relationship between frontal connectivity and EF, correlations were conducted between connectivity measures and KiTAP variables in participants with FXS. Given the strong association between IQ and EF, and to evaluate EF functions independent from general cognitive ability, partial Spearman correlations were adjusted for nonverbal IQ z-scores in order to limit the effect of impaired intellectual functioning on potential relationships between our neurophysiological and EF measures. This would allow us to interpret any significant corrections as a valid estimate of the relationship of phase connectivity with specific EF domain, independent from general cognitive functioning. Age also was included in partial correlations to account for wide age range sampled. Correlations were calculated across all FXS participants as well as for males and females separately. Due to the ceiling effect on KiTAP in TDC, correlations of EEG features and cognition were not examined in TDC.

Regression models

To further characterize the relationship between frontal connectivity and EF in individuals with FXS, generalized linear models were conducted to determine the best dWPLI predictors, if any, for each of the KiTAP scores adjusting for sex, nonverbal IQ, and age. The backward elimination variable selection technique was used to achieve the most parsimonious model. The criterion used to select the variables was based on the corrected Akaike information criterion (AICC), with a variable retained in the model if its p value < 0.05. If none of the dWPLI variables were selected, then separate models were examined for males and females with IQ forced in the model. Second-order interaction terms were also examined but none survived the variable selection procedure. Lastly, for each model selected, residuals were examined to see whether a different distribution (and its associated link function) was necessary to achieve a better fit. All correlations and regression modeling were conducted using SAS® software version 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC).

Results

Neural connectivity

Gamma band

A descriptive summary of gamma band functional connectivity (dWPLI) findings is presented in Fig. 1, with connections showing statistical significance for each region represented by color ribbons in Fig. 2. The full list of node pairs with significant case–control differences also is presented in Additional file 3: Tables S1–S5.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Visualization of frontal connectivity measured by dWPLI across FXS and TDC participants. Each data point represents the average of dWPLI values specific to a region and a frequency band within an individual subject. Boxplots show the 25, 50, and 75 percentiles (lower hinge, median, and upper hinge) per subgroup, separated by group and sex. Any data points beyond the whiskers, which are 1.5 times of IQR from the hinge, are regarded as outliers. Triangle markers denote a typically developing control (TDC), and circle markers denote a participant with Fragile X syndrome (FXS)

Fig. 2
figure 2

Circular chart of connections highlighting significant group differences in connectivity. Statistically significant group differences (multiple comparisons were FDR corrected per model) for band-specific connectivity depicted in regions a left frontal, b left prefrontal, c cross-hemisphere, d right frontal, and e right prefrontal. Male and females are shown separately when an interaction with sex was significant. Green ribbon denotes significant FXS > TDC connections, and red ribbon denotes significant FXS < TDC connections. Darker colors represent higher T-values. Atlas abbreviations: cMFG caudal middle frontal, FP frontal pole, LOF lateral orbitofrontal, MOF medial orbitofrontal, pOPER pars opercularis, pORB pars orbitalis, pTRI pars triangularis, rMFG rostral middle frontal, sFG superior frontal

Gamma band functional connectivity was significantly increased in all connections in FXS compared to TDC, indicating widespread elevations across all frontal regions in the patients. Linear modeling of gamma band dWPLI detected significant 3-way interactions (group/sex/connection) for left posterior frontal region (Fig. 3A; F(9, 7584.2) = 3.35, p = 0.0004), left prefrontal region (Fig. 3B; F(5, 4571.1) = 6.63, p < 0.0001), right posterior frontal region (Fig. 3D; F(9, 7563.0) = 2.98, p = 0.002), and cross-hemisphere connections (Fig. 3C; F(8, 6948.6) = 2.24, p = 0.0219). Post hoc pairwise comparisons of group in the left frontal region indicated that connectivity strength was greater in males with FXS compared to their sex-matched controls in pars opercularis (adj. p = 0.019) and pars triangularis (adj. p = 0.019). Similar connectivity strength elevations in females with FXS compared to TDC were observed between caudal middle frontal–superior frontal (adj. p = 0.019), pars opercularis–pars triangularis (adj. p = 0.028), pars opercularis–rostral middle frontal gyrus (adj. p = 0.028), and pars triangularis–rostral middle frontal gyrus (adj. p = 0.012).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Circular chart of connections highlighting significant group differences in connectivity after accounting for nonverbal IQ. Statistically significant group differences (multiple comparisons were FDR corrected per model) for band-specific connectivity depicted in regions a left frontal, b left prefrontal, c cross-hemisphere, d right frontal, and e right prefrontal. Male and females are shown separately when an interaction with sex was significant. Green ribbon denotes significant FXS > TDC connections, and red ribbon denotes significant FXS < TDC connections. Darker colors represent higher T values. Atlas abbreviations: cMFG caudal middle frontal, FP frontal pole, LOF lateral orbitofrontal, MOF medial orbitofrontal, pOPER pars opercularis, pORB pars orbitalis, pTRI pars triangularis, rMFG rostral middle frontal, sFG superior frontal

In right prefrontal cortex (Fig. 2E), we detected a 3-way interaction of gamma band activity in group/sex/frequency (F(5, 4559.0) = 3.87, p = 0.002) and a 2-way interaction of group/connection (F(5, 4559.4) = 4.95, p < 0.0002). Post hoc pairwise comparisons of group for group/connection showed increased connectivity strength in FXS compared to TDC in frontal pole–lateral orbitofrontal (adj. p = 0.0185), lateral orbitofrontal–medial orbitofrontal (adj. p < 0.0003), lateral orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis (adj. p < 0.0003), and medial orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis (adj. p = 0.006) as shown in Additional file 3: Table S4.2. Interestingly, the 3-way interaction of group/sex/frequency showed increased gamma connectivity strengths in males with FXS compared to sex-matched controls in the low gamma band range (30 Hz p = 0.04, 35 Hz p = 0.0001, 40 Hz p = 0.005). Increased connectivity strength also was found in females with FXS compared to their control counterparts in higher gamma band ranges (45 Hz p = 0.02, 50 Hz p = 0.02, 55 Hz p = 0.01).

Accounting for IQ

After accounting for nonverbal IQ (Additional file 3: Tables S1–S5), males with FXS no longer showed significantly increased connectivity strength compared to male controls in both left (Fig. 3A) and right (Fig. 3D) frontal regions. However, females with FXS demonstrated increased connectivity strength relative to their TDC counterparts in left frontal regions, specifically in connections caudal middle frontal–superior frontal (adj. p = 0.032), pars opercularis–superior frontal (adj. p = 0.024), and pars triangularis–rostral middle frontal (adj. p = 0.002). In the left prefrontal region (Fig. 3B), significantly increased connectivity strength in FXS relative to TDC remained. Female with FXS had two unique connections frontal pole–lateral orbitofrontal (adj. p < 0.0001) and frontal pole–pars orbitalis. In the right frontal region, females with FXS showed increased connectivity strength compared to control females in connections caudal middle frontal–superior frontal (adj. p = 0.031) and pars opercularis–pars triangularis (adj. p = 0.031). In the right prefrontal region (Fig. 3E), the FXS group demonstrated increased connectivity strength compared to TDC across all connections. In the model for cross-hemisphere connections (Fig. 3C), increased connectivity strength was observed in FXS relative to TDC, though sexes differed slightly in which connections were found to be significant.

Accounting for stimulant use

In FXS participants, the use of stimulants did not show any effect on gamma band connectivity strength across connections.

Sex differences in FXS

We further investigated group difference between sexes (i.e., male (FXS-TDC)–female (FXS-TDC)) for each EEG frequency, but failed to show significance, indicating that the degree of change in males did not differ significantly from those seen in females. Additionally, when examining the FXS group only, no sex differences emerged for any connection in the post hoc pairwise comparison, even after accounting for nonverbal IQ. To confirm findings, we removed the 10 males with mosaicism, as this subgroup tends to have intermediate phenotypes between males with full mutation FXS and females with FXS. Again, no sex differences were found to be significant in the post hoc pairwise comparison, even after accounting for nonverbal IQ.

Alpha band

A subject-level descriptive summary of upper alpha band dWPLI values by group and region is presented in Fig. 1. Connections showing statistical significance in alpha band for each region are represented by color ribbons in Fig. 2. Post hoc group comparisons showed reduced connectivity strength for lateral orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis and medial orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis in FXS relative to TDC across sexes (Additional file 3: Tables S6–7). Upper alpha band functional connectivity was reduced in FXS in right posterior frontal and prefrontal regions relative to controls. Of note, group differences in the upper alpha band were lateralized in the right hemisphere only. In right posterior frontal region (Fig. 2D), we detected a significant group/sex/connection 3-way interaction (F(9, 6403.2) = 4.72, p < 0.0001). In the post hoc group comparisons, connectivity strength for the pars triangularis–superior frontal connection was reduced in FXS relative to TDC in both sexes. Alpha band right prefrontal region model also detected a significant 2-way interaction of group/connection (Fig. 2E; F(5, 3806.2) = 13.43, p < 0.0001).

Accounting for IQ

After adjusting for nonverbal IQ (Additional file 3: Table S8), group differences in the right frontal region no longer reached significance. However, participants with FXS still demonstrated reduced connectivity strength compared to TDC in the right prefrontal region (Fig. 3E; lateral orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis adj. p = 0.0384).

Accounting for stimulant use

For participants with FXS, effects of stimulants on alpha band connectivity differed by sex (Additional file 3: Tables S9–11). In general, females taking stimulants demonstrated reduced alpha band connectivity compared to females not taking stimulants. This pattern was observed in the right frontal and the right prefrontal regions (pars opercularis–pars triangularis, adj. p = 0.044; lateral orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis, adj. p = 0.0232; medial orbitofrontal–pars orbitalis, adj. p = 0.0036). In contrast, males taking stimulant showed increased alpha band connectivity compared to males not taking stimulant. Specifically, this finding was observed in the right frontal region (rostral middle frontal–superior frontal, adj. p = 0.044), right prefrontal region (lateral orbitofrontal–medial orbitofrontal, adj. p = 0.0232) as well as in the cross-hemispheric connections (pars triangularis, adj. p = 0.0018).

Sex differences in FXS

Within alpha band, males with FXS showed reduced connectivity strength compared to females with FXS subjects at rostral middle frontal among cross-hemispheric connections (adj. p = 0.0027). This relationship remained significant after accounting for IQ (adj. p = 0.0054). After removing the mosaic males from analysis, the sex difference in the cross-hemispheric connection remained (adj. p = 0.0477), but not after accounting for nonverbal IQ (Additional file 3: Table S12).

Executive function

As expected, results from the KiTAP indicate that individuals with FXS have a lower performance overall across EF tasks as indicated by longer median response time and increased number of errors (Table 2). Across KiTAP variables, participants with FXS taking stimulants at the time of testing did not differ from their FXS counterparts not taking stimulants (Additional file 4: Tables S13–15).

Table 2 Summary of KiTAP performance for FXS and TDC participants

Relationships between neural connectivity and executive function

Spearman correlations

No significant correlations between IQ and dWPLI values in either frequency band were found in FXS.

Table 3 gives significant Spearman correlations between dWPLI and KiTAP variables adjusted for nonverbal IQ and age (see Additional file 5: Tables S16–17 for all correlations). Across FXS participants, we found significant relationships between increased error rates on the Distractibility and Go/NoGo tasks and increased gamma band connectivity strength in left posterior frontal and right posterior frontal regions (Fig. 4A). In alpha band, we found that reduced error rate during Flexibility was related to increased dWPLI connectivity in right prefrontal regions (Fig. 4B).

Table 3 Significant Spearman’s partial correlations (accounting for nonverbal IQ and age) between connectivity strength and executive function in FXS
Fig. 4
figure 4

Correlations between frontal connectivity and executive function measures across FXS. Spearman’s correlations showing positive correlation in the gamma band (A) and negative correlation in alpha band (B) between connectivity strength and number of errors

Relationships between gamma band connectivity and error rates were primarily driven by males with FXS (Fig. 5A). We also found that increased connectivity strength in the gamma band was associated with shorter response times during the Flexibility task in males with FXS (Fig. 5B). Unexpectedly, in females with FXS, we found that increased gamma band cross-hemispheric and right prefrontal connectivity strength were associated with fewer Flexibility errors (Fig. 5C).

Fig. 5
figure 5

Correlations between frontal connectivity and executive function measures by sex. Sex-specific significant Spearman’s correlations are shown separately for males (A, B) and females (C) for both gamma and alpha bands

Regression models

Significant regression models are provided in Table 4, which give the best fitting models for KiTAP scores as a linear function of selected dWPLI measures after controlling for sex, age, and nonverbal IQ. For each model, the band-specific dWPLI measure served as the dependent factor. For each significant model included in Table 4, if no distribution is noted, then the default Gaussian model was used. If a distribution is noted, then its residual profile is a better fit.

Table 4 Significant regression models in FXS

Briefly, in the gamma band, for every 0.01 increase in dWPLI in the left frontal region, Distractor error increased by approximately 16%, and Flexibility median reaction time decreased by approximately 10%. The latter is the only model in which age was significant, such that each one year increase in age, Flexibility median reaction decreases by approximately 1.9%. For females only, every 0.01 increase in dWPLI in the left posterior frontal region corresponds to an increase in 1.75 errors during Flexibility, and every 0.1 increase in cross-hemispheric (i.e., more rightward) dWPLI corresponds to a decrease in 2.95 errors during Flexibility. Further, each 0.01 increase in dWPLI in the right prefrontal region in alpha band corresponded to a decrease in 0.61 errors during the Flexibility task, and every 0.01 increase in dWPLI in the right posterior frontal region Distractor error decreased by approximately 9%.

Discussion

Resting state EEG phase connectivity within source-localized frontal regions in a well-powered sample of individuals with FXS and matched controls revealed two key findings. First, individuals with FXS broadly demonstrated increased gamma and reduced alpha phase connectivity across frontal regions, both within and across hemispheres, compared to TDC. Second, significant associations between EF and frontal connectivity emerged in FXS, such that increased error rates were associated with increased gamma connectivity strength and inversely associated with reduced alpha connectivity strength. Notably, these correlational findings remained robust after accounting for general cognitive functioning and age. We document an important and not previously reported link between deficits in EF and the alterations in the coherence of specific frequencies of neural oscillations within the frontal cortices of individuals with FXS. Together, our study reveals a potential underlying neurophysiological basis for EF impairment in FXS that may represent a promising target for future intervention studies.

Phase connectivity

Phase connectivity, when applied spatially, assesses the precise alignment of neural oscillations at a specific frequency between brain regions. Phase connectivity, or coherence, between different brain regions is well known to support cognitive functions [43, 45, 50]. Herein, we implemented source localization to examine point-to-point connectivity within frontal regions to enhance regional specificity of previously reported phase connectivity alterations and their functional significance [14, 16, 23]. Previous investigations into other neurodevelopmental and neurological disorders have linked alterations in resting frontal phase connectivity with impaired cognitive function, including deficits in EF [51,52,53].

Our primary results are consistent with our and other’s previous electrode-level phase connectivity findings, but further localize a subset of regions with gamma band hyper-connectivity and alpha band hypo-connectivity within frontal cortex in individuals with FXS [14, 16, 23]. Specifically, gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity may reflect poor top-down regulation of local frontal circuits leading to hyperexcitability of local circuit function and subsequent cognitive and behavioral dysfunction [54, 55]. The observed alpha hypo-connectivity may represent deficient longer-range inhibitory mechanisms which down-regulate background neural excitability [42]. From the perspective of an excitatory–inhibitory imbalance (E:I) model of neurodevelopmental disorders, our current findings are consistent with a hyper-excitable phenotype that has been repeatedly documented in FXS across in vivo slice physiology and mouse model studies [17, 19, 21, 56].

More recently, a growing body of the literature has raised the importance of increased variability in neural signals linked to enhanced cognition, as systems need to be tuned on-line to optimize them for behavioral demands [57,58,59]. Our observations, when taken together with other EEG findings in FXS (e.g., decreased peak alpha frequency, decreased neural synchronization to the auditory chirp, reduction in global alpha power with concomitant increases in regional gamma power, reduced EEG signal complexity), suggest a diminished capacity or increased constraints on the expression of neural variability in the FXS cortex [15, 22, 60,61,62,63]. Elevated gamma band connectivity and suppressed alpha band connectivity at rest, as found here in individuals with FXS, could reflect a neural state patients are seemingly “stuck” in, thus resulting in reduced ability to dynamically adjust neural responses to environmental demands, especially those requiring higher cognitive skills. From a molecular standpoint, loss of FMRP results in a reduction of synaptic plasticity, deficits in stimulus-induced synaptic protein synthesis, synaptic overgrowth, and changes in dendritic spine morphology in the FMR1−/− KO mouse and neurons derived from FXS patients [64,65,66,67,68]. Such changes also would be predicted to dampen neural variability at the molecular and cellular levels [60]. Since our findings were relatively similar across sexes in FXS, this suggests a general reduction in FMRP, as opposed to a complete loss, may be sufficient to alter frontal connectivity and, in turn, EF.

Evolving model of EF physiology in FXS

The association between EF task performance and frontal lobe phase connectivity in the present study can be used to advance our understanding of higher-order cognitive processes in FXS and, for the first time, establish a neurophysiological model of impaired EF in this patient population. Importantly, the correlation and regression findings remained robust, even after correcting for general intellectual functioning and age. This suggests that frontal gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity may be specifically related to EF deficits rather than intellectual or general cognitive capacity more broadly. Alterations to alpha and gamma phase connectivity predicted increased error rates during a distractibility task as well as increased error rates and reduced reaction time during a cognitive flexibility task. Although faster reaction times are often thought to indicate more intact cognitive processes, this is not necessarily the case in the context of EF when slower reaction times can be beneficial in terms of the speed/accuracy trade-off [48, 69].

We speculate that functional consequences of connectivity abnormalities may include poorer local regulation of frontal activity in combination with deficient inhibitory mechanisms, thus leading to difficulty in cognitive flexibility, attention shifting, ignoring distractions, and increased impulsivity. Moreover, our findings are consistent with the canonical role of alpha oscillations in attention and cognition, such that enhanced alpha frontal connectivity helps facilitate shifting attention and cognitive resources to support behavioral flexibility and inhibit distraction from sensory information that is not relevant to contextual demands [37, 42]. Thus, our findings suggest increased alpha band connectivity may support EF in a compensatory fashion. Previous fMRI studies have implicated compensatory mechanisms of increased activation in prefrontal regions to support inhibitory control and prevent distractor interference in FXS [13, 70]. Complementing the general activation finding from fMRI work, our EEG study highlights the breadth and spatial distance of altered coherence of neural oscillation across regions that occurs in specific frequency bands that have their own functional significance.

It is important to note that several of our group differences in gamma band connectivity were no longer significant after accounting for nonverbal IQ, which seemed to be particular to males with FXS in right and left frontal regions. As prefrontal regions are more synonymous with their role in EF, this suggests prefrontal gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity may be more specific to FXS pathology rather than that associated with general intellectual disability. In contrast, group differences in gamma band connectivity in posterior frontal regions may be more dependent on FMRP expression, which we know are significantly different in FXS and TDC males and correspond to IQ score in FXS males [71]. Future studies examining the relationship between resting state connectivity and FMRP levels are needed to clarify this finding.

Sex differences not observed in FXS

Contrary to our expectations, frontal gamma hyper-connectivity and alpha hypo-connectivity were mostly similarly effected in males and females with FXS. As full mutation males with FXS have significantly less expression of FMRP [71] and a higher burden of clinical symptoms [4], we had predicted connectivity alterations would be more prominent in males than females. Previous studies have replicated the finding of increased resting local gamma power in males with FXS [15, 22, 60, 72]. However, it is important to note that phase-based measures, such as dWPLI, can show increases in phase synchronized neural oscillations across regions rather than just a parallel increase in the power at a given frequency band. Thus, our finding indicate high-frequency activity in local circuits may be restricted to males with FXS, whereas high-frequency activity in the mutual synchronized driving of excitability across widely distributed brain regions may be more broadly present across males and females with FXS.

Interestingly, sex differences in FXS emerged when comparing participants taking stimulants at the time of testing versus those who were not. Males with FXS taking stimulants had increased alpha band connectivity in a few connections within frontal and prefrontal regions compared to males taking not stimulants. In contrast, females with FXS taking stimulants had reduced alpha band connectivity compared to females not taking stimulants. One possible interpretation of this finding is that stimulants may help “normalize” alpha band connectivity in males with FXS. Given that partial correlations showed increases in alpha band connectivity corresponding to reductions in errors during the Flexibility and Distractibility tasks, especially among males, our findings implicate a possible mechanism by which stimulant medication reduces impulsivity in FXS males.

Although the majority of ADHD studies have not found a difference in frontal connectivity based on whether participants were taking stimulants or not [73,74,75], at least one study reported frontal connectivity in the alpha band was reduced within the stimulant group compared to the non-stimulant group, consistent with our findings in females with FXS [76]. Thus, it is possible that stimulants may suppress alpha band connectivity within females with FXS. Alternatively, it is possible that females with FXS who take stimulants have lower alpha band connectivity at baseline than females who do not, and the stimulants themselves do not change alpha band connectivity. Future studies are needed to determine the impact of stimulant medication on frontal functional connectivity and EF in FXS.

Evidence of atypical lateralization

Lateralized substrates for distinct cognitive functions within frontal cortex have been consistently observed in typically developing individuals. For example, right inferior and superior frontal gyri have been implicated in inhibitory control, specifically proactive control related to reaction time slowing [77, 78]. Atypical brain lateralization of cognitive functions has been observed in other neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD [79, 80]. Our findings add to these previous studies by documenting atypical lateralization in individuals with FXS. For example, we found increased gamma band, but reduced alpha band, in right prefrontal regions in individuals with FXS compared to TDC. This finding suggests EF skills lateralized within these regions would be affected, which our partial correlation and regression findings support. Specifically, we observed reduced alpha connectivity within these atypically lateralized regions predicted impaired performance during cognitive flexibility and distractibility tasks. Yet, consistent with our compensatory hypothesis, our findings also suggest that preserved lateralized right frontal alpha connectivity may facilitate inhibition of previously learned behavior (flexibility) and irrelevant sensory stimuli (distractibility) in individuals with FXS, especially among females. This interpretation is consistent with our recent finding showing preserved lateralization of low gamma activity was related to reduced inhibition errors [27].

Notably, among males with FXS, we found gamma hyper-connectivity in left pars opercularis and pars triangularis, areas within the inferior frontal gyrus which are critical for speech and language. This suggests increased phase synchronized gamma activity within these frontal regions may contribute to language impairments and delays in FXS, which are nearly universal among male patients [81, 82]. Our previous work has shown that prior to the onset of speech production individuals with FXS demonstrate increased frontal gamma power compared to controls, and greater increases in this phasic gamma power were associated with more unintelligible speech in FXS [83]. Yet, this finding in males was no longer significant after controlling for nonverbal IQ. Thus, taken all together, these findings implicate increased local and synchronized high-frequency frontal activity may have widespread disruptive role in FXS that is not necessarily specific to EF or speech production. Future studies are needed to determine the extent to which high-frequency activity within frontal cortices more broadly affects learning and development in FXS.

Limitations

Frontal connectivity findings are limited to brain activity at rest and thus should not be equated with task-based connectivity findings. Similarly, resting connectivity findings may not generalize to neural activity during real world function. Thus, future work is needed to study dynamic changes in neural oscillation during EF task performance. Findings further are limited to short-range frontal connectivity and do not consider longer-range connectivity relevant to EF (e.g., fronto-parietal connections). Still, findings remain the first of their kind in FXS and represent a critical step to better understanding neurophysiological mechanisms underlying impaired EF in FXS. It also is important to note that only certain aspects of EF were measured using KiTAP, indicating the need to replicate findings in a broader battery of neuropsychological tests (e.g., NIH Cognitive Toolbox). We did not have an IQ- and age-matched control group, and although the majority of our findings remained after controlling for IQ, inclusion of such a control group in addition to examining relationships with FMRP levels is needed in future studies to determine the extent to which our findings are specific to FXS pathology. Similarly, we note that FMRP expression is not dichotomous based on sex as presently described. Thus, future work examining frontal connectivity in relation to a continuous measure of FMRP [71] is needed to better understand the role of FMRP in EF impairments in FXS. Our use of a validated source localization method without the use of MRI was a strength of our study; however, to confirm regional findings future studies would benefit from concomitant MRI acquisition. Another strength of our study was the inclusion of a wide age range of participants; however, we did not have sufficient data across ages to include as an additional factor in our primary connectivity models. Future studies with larger samples are needed to explore the developmental trajectory of frontal functional connectivity and executive function skills in FXS. Last, due to the significant overlap of EF impairments in FXS and ADHD, it also will be important in future work to determine whether frontal functional connectivity patterns differ between individuals with FXS who have comorbid ADHD or not.

Conclusions

In the first study of its kind using high-density source-localized resting state EEG, we documented increased gamma band connectivity and reduced alpha band connectivity in frontal brain regions in individuals with FXS relative to TDC, and these connectivity abnormalities were predictive of executive function deficits in FXS, independent of general cognitive ability. Our findings implicate gamma hyper-connectivity within frontal brain regions and thus support and extend previous findings demonstrating E:I imbalance in FXS. Given the directions of correlation, we hypothesize that increased gamma connectivity may impair EF performance via its relation to hyperexcitability of cortex, whereas increased alpha connectivity may provide compensatory support for EF in individuals with FXS by facilitating adaptive shifts is brain state needed for context-relevant behavioral demands. Together, our findings provide novel insight into potential mechanisms of deficit in EF in FXS and suggest that frontal phase connectivity may be an important measure of target engagement in future intervention trials.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset used for the current is available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

CGG:

Cysteine-guanine-guanine

CH:

Cross-hemisphere

cMFG:

Caudal middle frontal

dWPLI:

Debiased weighted phase lag index

EEG:

Electroencephalography

EF:

Executive function

E:I:

Excitatory/inhibitory

FP:

Frontal pole

FMR1:

Fragile X messenger ribonucleoprotein 1 (FMR1)

fMRI:

Functional magnetic resonance imaging

FMRP:

Fragile X protein

FXS:

Fragile X syndrome

IAPF:

Individual alpha peak frequency

ID:

Intellectual disability

IQ:

Intelligence quotient

KiTAP:

Test of Attentional Performance in Children

LF:

Left Frontal

lme:

Linear mixed effect

LOF:

Lateral orbitofrontal

LPF:

Left prefrontal

MOF:

Medial orbitofrontal

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

PCR:

Polymerase chain reaction

pORB:

Pars orbitalis

pOPER:

Pars opercularis

pTRI:

Pars triangularis

RF:

Right frontal

rMFG:

Rostral middle frontal gyrus

RPF:

Right prefrontal

RT:

Response time

SB-5:

Stanford-Binet-5th Edition

SD:

Standard deviation

sFG:

Superior frontal

TDC:

Typically developing control

References

  1. Garber KB, Visootsak J, Warren ST. Fragile X syndrome. Eur J Hum Genet. 2008;16(6):666–72.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Protic D, Salcedo-Arellano MJ, Dy JB, Potter LA, Hagerman RJ. New Targeted treatments for fragile X syndrome. Curr Pediatr Rev. 2019;15(4):251–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Schmitt LM, Shaffer RC, Hessl D, Erickson C. Executive function in fragile X syndrome: a systematic review. Brain Sci. 2019;9(1):15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Baker EK, Arpone M, Vera SA, Bretherton L, Ure A, Kraan CM, et al. Intellectual functioning and behavioural features associated with mosaicism in fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2019;11(1):41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Meng L, Kaufmann WE, Frye RE, Ong K, Kaminski JW, Velinov M, et al. The association between mosaicism type and cognitive and behavioral functioning among males with fragile X syndrome. Am J Med Genet A. 2022;188(3):858–66.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  6. Bishop SL, Richler J, Cain AC, Lord C. Predictors of perceived negative impact in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Ment Retard. 2007;112(6):450–61.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Lewis P, Abbeduto L, Murphy M, Richmond E, Giles N, Bruno L, et al. Cognitive, language and social-cognitive skills of individuals with fragile X syndrome with and without autism. J Intellect Disabil Res. 2006;50(Pt 7):532–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Weber JD, Smith E, Berry-Kravis E, Cadavid D, Hessl D, Erickson C. Voice of people with fragile X syndrome and their families: reports from a survey on treatment priorities. Brain Sci. 2019;9(2):18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Alvarez JA, Emory E. Executive function and the frontal lobes: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychol Rev. 2006;16(1):17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Hallahan BP, Craig MC, Toal F, Daly EM, Moore CJ, Ambikapathy A, et al. In vivo brain anatomy of adult males with fragile X syndrome: an MRI study. Neuroimage. 2011;54(1):16–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Gothelf D, Furfaro JA, Hoeft F, Eckert MA, Hall SS, O’Hara R, et al. Neuroanatomy of fragile X syndrome is associated with aberrant behavior and the fragile X mental retardation protein (FMRP). Ann Neurol. 2008;63(1):40–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Kwon H, Menon V, Eliez S, Warsofsky IS, White CD, Dyer-Friedman J, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of visuospatial working memory in fragile X syndrome: relation to behavioral and molecular measures. Am J Psychiatry. 2001;158(7):1040–51.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Hoeft F, Hernandez A, Parthasarathy S, Watson CL, Hall SS, Reiss AL. Fronto-striatal dysfunction and potential compensatory mechanisms in male adolescents with fragile X syndrome. Hum Brain Mapp. 2007;28(6):543–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Esteban FJ, van der Molen MJW, Stam CJ, van der Molen MW. Resting-state EEG oscillatory dynamics in fragile X syndrome: abnormal functional connectivity and brain network organization. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e88451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  15. Smith EG, Pedapati EV, Liu R, Schmitt LM, Dominick KC, Shaffer RC, et al. Sex differences in resting EEG power in fragile X syndrome. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;138:89–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Wang J, Ethridge LE, Mosconi MW, White SP, Binder DK, Pedapati EV, et al. A resting EEG study of neocortical hyperexcitability and altered functional connectivity in fragile X syndrome. J Neurodev Disord. 2017;9(1):1–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Lovelace JW, Ethell IM, Binder DK, Razak KA. Translation-relevant EEG phenotypes in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2018;115:39–48.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Pedapati EV, Schmitt LM, Ethridge LE, Miyakoshi M, Sweeney JA, Liu R, et al. Neocortical localization and thalamocortical modulation of neuronal hyperexcitability in fragile X syndrome. Commun Biol. 2022;5:1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Goswami S, Cavalier S, Sridhar V, Huber KM, Gibson JR. Local cortical circuit correlates of altered EEG in the mouse model of Fragile X syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2019;124:563–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Jonak CR, Lovelace JW, Ethell IM, Razak KA, Binder DK. Multielectrode array analysis of EEG biomarkers in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2020;138:104794.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Gibson JR, Bartley AF, Hays SA, Huber KM. Imbalance of neocortical excitation and inhibition and altered UP states reflect network hyperexcitability in the mouse model of fragile X syndrome. J Neurophysiol. 2008;100(5):2615–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Pedapati EV, Schmitt LM, Liu R, Ethridge LE, Smith E, Sweeney JA, et al. Neocortical localization and thalamocortical modulation of neuronal hyperexcitability in fragile X syndrome. medRxiv Preprint Server for Health Sciences. 2021.

  23. van der Molen MJ, Stam CJ, van der Molen MW. Resting-state EEG oscillatory dynamics in fragile X syndrome: abnormal functional connectivity and brain network organization. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(2):e88451.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Jones DT, Graff-Radford J. Executive dysfunction and the prefrontal cortex. CONTINUUM Lifelong Learn Neurol. 2021;27(6):1586–601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Friedman NP, Robbins TW. The role of prefrontal cortex in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47(1):72–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Norris JE, DeStefano LA, Schmitt LM, Pedapati EV, Erickson CA, Sweeney JA, et al. Hemispheric utilization of alpha oscillatory dynamics as a unique biomarker of neural compensation in females with fragile X syndrome. ACS Chem Neurosci. 2022. https://doi.org/10.1021/acschemneuro.2c00404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Roid GH, Pomplun M. The Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, fifth edition. Contemporary intellectual assessment: theories, tests, and issues. 3rd ed. New York: The Guilford Press; 2012. p. 249–68.

    Google Scholar 

  29. Sansone SM, Schneider A, Bickel E, Berry-Kravis E, Prescott C, Hessl D. Improving IQ measurement in intellectual disabilities using true deviation from population norms. J Neurodev Disord. 2014;6(1):16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Ethridge LE, White SP, Mosconi MW, Wang J, Pedapati EV, Erickson CA, et al. Neural synchronization deficits linked to cortical hyper-excitability and auditory hypersensitivity in fragile X syndrome. Mol Autism. 2017;8:22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Tadel F, Baillet S, Mosher JC, Pantazis D, Leahy RM. Brainstorm: a user-friendly application for MEG/EEG analysis. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:879716.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Desikan RS, Segonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Elliott R. Executive functions and their disorders: imaging in clinical neuroscience. Br Med Bull. 2003;65(1):49–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Gertel VH, Zhang H, Diaz MT. Stronger right hemisphere functional connectivity supports executive aspects of language in older adults. Brain Lang. 2020;206:104771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Margolis A, Donkervoort M, Kinsbourne M, Peterson BS. Interhemispheric connectivity and executive functioning in adults with Tourette syndrome. Neuropsychology. 2006;20(1):66–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Brzezicka A, Kamiński J, Kamińska OK, Wołyńczyk-Gmaj D, Sedek G. Frontal EEG alpha band asymmetry as a predictor of reasoning deficiency in depressed people. Cogn Emot. 2017;31(5):868–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(12):606–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Palva S, Palva JM. Functional roles of alpha-band phase synchronization in local and large-scale cortical networks. Front Psychol. 2011;2:204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Roux F, Wibral M, Mohr HM, Singer W, Uhlhaas PJ. Gamma-band activity in human prefrontal cortex codes for the number of relevant items maintained in working memory. J Neurosci. 2012;32(36):12411–20.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Bosman CA, Lansink CS, Pennartz CM. Functions of gamma-band synchronization in cognition: from single circuits to functional diversity across cortical and subcortical systems. Eur J Neurosci. 2014;39(11):1982–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Huang MX, Huang CW, Harrington DL, Nichols S, Robb-Swan A, Angeles-Quinto A, et al. Marked increases in resting-state MEG gamma-band activity in combat-related mild traumatic brain injury. Cereb Cortex. 2020;30(1):283–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Klimesch W, Sauseng P, Hanslmayr S. EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition–timing hypothesis. Brain Res Rev. 2007;53(1):63–88.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Vinck M, Oostenveld R, van Wingerden M, Battaglia F, Pennartz CMA. An improved index of phase-synchronization for electrophysiological data in the presence of volume-conduction, noise and sample-size bias. Neuroimage. 2011;55(4):1548–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Cohen MX. Analyzing neural time series data: theory and practice. Grafman J, editor. Cambridge: The MIT Press; 2014.

  45. Vindiola MM, Vettel JM, Gordon SM, Franaszczuk PJ, McDowell K. Applying EEG phase synchronization measures to non-linearly coupled neural mass models. J Neurosci Methods. 2014;226:1–14.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Testsysteme P. KiTAP test of attentional performance for children. 2011.

  47. Knox A, Schneider A, Abucayan F, Hervey C, Tran C, Hessl D, et al. Feasibility, reliability, and clinical validity of the Test of Attentional Performance for Children (KiTAP) in Fragile X syndrome (FXS). J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4(1):2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Schmitt LM, White SP, Cook EH, Sweeney JA, Mosconi MW. Cognitive mechanisms of inhibitory control deficits in autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2017;59:586–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Bates D, Machler M, Bolker B, Walker S. Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J Stat Softw. 2015;67:1–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Fries P. Rhythms for cognition: communication through coherence. Neuron. 2015;88(1):220–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  51. Hata M, Kazui H, Tanaka T, Ishii R, Canuet L, Pascual-Marqui RD, et al. Functional connectivity assessed by resting state EEG correlates with cognitive decline of Alzheimer’s disease—an eLORETA study. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127(2):1269–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Machinskaya RI, Semenova OA, Absatova KA, Sugrobova GA. Neurophysiological factors associated with cognitive deficits in children with ADHD symptoms: EEG and neuropsychological analysis. Psychol Neurosci. 2014;7:461–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Xu P, Xiong XC, Xue Q, Tian Y, Peng Y, Zhang R, et al. Recognizing mild cognitive impairment based on network connectivity analysis of resting EEG with zero reference. Physiol Meas. 2014;35(7):1279–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Donner TH, Siegel M. A framework for local cortical oscillation patterns. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(5):191–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  55. Cabral J, Kringelbach ML, Deco G. Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Prog Neurobiol. 2014;114:102–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Lovelace JW, Wen TH, Reinhard S, Hsu MS, Sidhu H, Ethell IM, et al. Matrix metalloproteinase-9 deletion rescues auditory evoked potential habituation deficit in a mouse model of fragile X syndrome. Neurobiol Dis. 2016;89:126–35.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  57. Waschke L, Kloosterman NA, Obleser J, Garrett DD. Behavior needs neural variability. Neuron. 2021;109(5):751–66.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  58. Kloosterman NA, Kosciessa JQ, Lindenberger U, Fahrenfort JJ, Garrett DD. Boosts in brain signal variability track liberal shifts in decision bias. Elife. 2020;9:e54201.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  59. Garrett DD, Epp SM, Kleemeyer M, Lindenberger U, Polk TA. Higher performers upregulate brain signal variability in response to more feature-rich visual input. Neuroimage. 2020;217:116836.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  60. Wilkinson CL, Nelson CA. Increased aperiodic gamma power in young boys with fragile X syndrome is associated with better language ability. Mol Autism. 2021;12(1):17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  61. Ethridge LE, De Stefano LA, Schmitt LM, Woodruff NE, Brown KL, Tran M, et al. Auditory EEG biomarkers in fragile X syndrome: clinical relevance. Front Integr Neurosci. 2019;13:60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Côté V, Lalancette È, Knoth IS, Côté L, Agbogba K, Vannasing P, et al. Distinct patterns of repetition suppression in Fragile X syndrome, down syndrome, tuberous sclerosis complex and mutations in SYNGAP1. Brain Res. 2021;1751:147205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  63. Proteau-Lemieux M, Knoth IS, Agbogba K, Côté V, Barlahan Biag HM, Thurman AJ, et al. EEG signal complexity is reduced during resting-state in fragile X syndrome. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:716707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. He CX, Portera-Cailliau C. The trouble with spines in fragile X syndrome: density, maturity and plasticity. Neuroscience. 2013;251:120–8.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  65. Zhang Z, Marro SG, Zhang Y, Arendt KL, Patzke C, Zhou B, et al. The fragile X mutation impairs homeostatic plasticity in human neurons by blocking synaptic retinoic acid signaling. Sci Transl Med. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1126/scitranslmed.aar4338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Comery TA, Harris JB, Willems PJ, Oostra BA, Irwin SA, Weiler IJ, et al. Abnormal dendritic spines in fragile X knockout mice: maturation and pruning deficits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 1997;94(10):5401–4.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  67. Doll CA, Broadie K. Activity-dependent FMRP requirements in development of the neural circuitry of learning and memory. Development. 2015;142(7):1346–56.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  68. Kennedy T, Rinker D, Broadie K. Genetic background mutations drive neural circuit hyperconnectivity in a fragile X syndrome model. BMC Biol. 2020;18(1):94.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  69. Schmitt LM, Ankeny LD, Sweeney JA, Mosconi MW. Inhibitory control processes and the strategies that support them during hand and eye movements. Front Psychol. 2016;7:1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Martin A, Quintin EM, Hall SS, Reiss AL. The role of executive function in independent living skills in female adolescents and young adults with fragile X syndrome. Am J Intellect Dev Disabil. 2016;121(5):448–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Boggs AE, Schmitt LM, McLane RD, Adayev T, LaFauci G, Horn PS, et al. Optimization, validation and initial clinical implications of a Luminex-based immunoassay for the quantification of fragile X protein from dried blood spots. Sci Rep. 2022;12(1):5617.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  72. Wang X, Snape M, Klann E, Stone JG, Singh A, Petersen RB, et al. Activation of the extracellular signal-regulated kinase pathway contributes to the behavioral deficit of fragile x-syndrome. J Neurochem. 2012;121(4):672–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  73. Dupuy FE, Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M. EEG coherence in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: stimulant effects in good responders. Int J Psychophysiol. 2008;70(3):151–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Clarke AR, Barry RJ, McCarthy R, Selikowitz M, Johnstone SJ, Abbott I, et al. Effects of methylphenidate on EEG coherence in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Int J Psychophysiol. 2005;58(1):4–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  75. Michelini G, Jurgiel J, Bakolis I, Cheung CHM, Asherson P, Loo SK, et al. Atypical functional connectivity in adolescents and adults with persistent and remitted ADHD during a cognitive control task. Transl Psychiatry. 2019;9(1):137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  76. Murias M, Swanson JM, Srinivasan R. Functional connectivity of frontal cortex in healthy and ADHD children reflected in EEG coherence. Cereb Cortex. 2007;17(8):1788–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Aron AR, Poldrack RA. Cortical and subcortical contributions to Stop signal response inhibition: role of the subthalamic nucleus. J Neurosci. 2006;26(9):2424–33.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  78. Hu S, Ide JS, Zhang S, Li CR. The right superior frontal gyrus and individual variation in proactive control of impulsive response. J Neurosci. 2016;36(50):12688–96.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  79. D’Cruz AM, Mosconi MW, Steele S, Rubin LH, Luna B, Minshew N, et al. Lateralized response timing deficits in autism. Biol Psychiatry. 2009;66(4):393–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. O’Reilly C, Lewis JD, Elsabbagh M. Is functional brain connectivity atypical in autism? A systematic review of EEG and MEG studies. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(5):e0175870.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. Abbeduto L, Brady N, Kover ST. Language development and fragile X syndrome: profiles, syndrome-specificity, and within-syndrome differences. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2007;13(1):36–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Roberts JE, Mankowski JB, Sideris J, Goldman BD, Hatton DD, Mirrett PL, et al. Trajectories and predictors of the development of very young boys with fragile X syndrome. J Pediatr Psychol. 2009;34(8):827–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Schmitt LM, Wang J, Pedapati EV, Thurman AJ, Abbeduto L, Erickson CA, et al. A neurophysiological model of speech production deficits in fragile X syndrome. Brain Commun. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1093/braincomms/fcz042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to acknowledge the individuals with FXS, their families, and the controls who participated in this study. We also want to thank the clinical research coordinators for their assistance with data collection and data entry. We also appreciate the support from Elizabeth Berry-Kravis at Rush University for testing for FMR1 gene CGG expansion and gene methylation.

Funding

This research study was supported by the U54 Fragile X Center (U54HD082008, J.A.S and U54HD104461, C.A.E) and K23 Mentored Patient-Oriented Research (K23HD101416, L.M.S).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

LMS interpreted data and wrote full draft of the manuscript. JL and RL analyzed the dWPLI data and assisted with manuscript draft. PH analyzed the correlation and regression data. JAS, CAE, and EVP assisted with data interpretation and edited the manuscript. JAS and CAE participated in the initial conceptualization of the broader study, and LMS, JL, and EVP conceptualized the specific sub-study. All authors reviewed the manuscript and approved the submitted version. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Lauren M. Schmitt.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Cincinnati Children’s Hospital Medical Center. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest for the current manuscript.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

A priori designated eighteen nodes within frontal regions used for analysis based on their known contributions to executive function.

Additional file 2.

Properties of dWPLI measure.

Additional file 3.

Post hoc pair comparisons of group from lme models for gamma and alpha band connectivity.

Additional file 4.

Summary of KiTAP performance of individuals with FXS taking stimulants (FXS+stimulant), those not taking stimulants (FXS-stimulant), and typically-developing controls (TDC).

Additional file 5.

Spearman’s Partial Correlations (Accounting for Nonverbal IQ and Age) Gamma Connectivity Strength and Executive Function as well as Alpha Connectivity Strength and Executive Function in FXS.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Schmitt, L.M., Li, J., Liu, R. et al. Altered frontal connectivity as a mechanism for executive function deficits in fragile X syndrome. Molecular Autism 13, 47 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00527-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00527-0

Keywords