Skip to main content

Oxytocin impacts top-down and bottom-up social perception in adolescents with ASD: a MEG study of neural connectivity

Abstract

Background

In the last decade, accumulative evidence has shown that oxytocin can modulate social perception in typically developed individuals and individuals diagnosed with autism. While several studies show that oxytocin (OT) modulates neural activation in social-related neural regions, the mechanism that underlies OT effects in ASD is not fully known yet. Despite evidence from animal studies on connections between the oxytocinergic system and excitation/inhibition neural balance, the influence of OT on oscillatory responses among individuals with ASD has been rarely examined. To bridge these gaps in knowledge, we investigated the effects of OT on both social and non-social stimuli while focusing on its specific influence on the neural connectivity between three socially related neural regions—the left and right fusiform and the medial frontal cortex.

Methods

Twenty-five adolescents with ASD participated in a wall-established social task during a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled MEG and OT administration study. Our main task was a social-related task that required the identification of social and non-social-related pictures. We hypothesized that OT would modulate the oscillatory connectivity between three pre-selected regions of interest to be more adaptive to social processing. Specifically, we focused on alpha and gamma bands which are known to play an important role in face processing and top-down/bottom-up balance.

Results

Compared to placebo, OT reduced the connectivity between the medial frontal cortex and the fusiform in the low gamma more for social stimuli than for non-social ones, a reduction that was correlated with individuals’ performance in the task. Additionally, for both social and non-social stimuli, OT increased the connectivity in the alpha and beta bands.

Limitations

Sample size was determined based on sample sizes previously reported in MEG in clinical populations, especially OT administration studies in combination with neuroimaging in ASD. We were limited in our capability to recruit for such a study, and as such, the sample size was not based on a priori power analysis. Additionally, we limited our analyses to specific neural bands and regions. To validate the current results, future studies may be needed to explore other parameters using whole-brain approaches in larger samples.

Conclusion

These results suggest that OT influenced social perception by modifying the communication between frontal and posterior regions, an attenuation that potentially impacts both social and non-social early perception. We also show that OT influences differ between top-down and bottom-up processes, depending on the social context. Overall, by showing that OT influences both social-related perception and overall attention during early processing stages, we add new information to the existing understanding of the impact of OT on neural processing in ASD. Furthermore, by highlighting the influence of OT on early perception, we provide new directions for treatments for difficulties in early attentional phases in this population.

Trial registration Registered on October 27, 2021—Retrospectively registered, https://clinicaltrials.gov/ct2/show/record/NCT05096676 (details on clinical registration can be found in www.clinicalTrial.gov, unique identifier: NCT05096676).

Introduction

ASD, face processing, and oscillatory neural activity

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by difficulties in the social domain, repetitive behaviors, and restricted thinking [1]. In addition to the diagnostic markers for ASD, several studies have shown that individuals with ASD may also present difficulties in face perception and emotional expression interpretation in a variety of tasks [2, 3]. One explanation for this phenomenon suggests that, compared to typically developed individuals (TD), individuals with ASD tend to focus more on the local details of the face instead of automatically perceiving it as one figure [4, 5].

The ability to identify emotional expression from a face requires a widespread neural network, which includes regions such as the fusiform face area, frontal regions, and occipital cortices [68], regions that tend to show atypical activation and connectivity patterns in individuals with ASD. Moreover, it has been suggested that the ability to process the information from facial expressions stems not only from the neural activation in these regions but also from the communication between them [912]. One way to conceptualize the relationship between face perception and communication between brain regions relates to the “top-down” and “bottom-up” perception. During face processing, one should balance between bottom-up processing, which focuses on the local features of the face, and top-down holistic interpretation by processing the face as one whole shape [13, 14], a balance that was suggested to be atypical in ASD [4, 5].

Using electro-and-magneto-encephalographic (EEG and MEG, respectively), studies have shown that the unique balance between top-down and bottom-up perception is modulated by the gamma-band frequency power and synchronization, a neural oscillation with a frequency between 30 and 100 Hz [15]. While long-range connectivity in low frequencies (alpha band, 8–13 Hz) is often correlated with a priori knowledge and top-down processing [16], the power and connectivity in gamma represent stimulus-related sensory information, local feature processing [17], and cognitive binding of features [18, 19]—all relevant functions for face processing. Indeed, studies have shown that an increase in gamma power and its synchronization between- and within-regions that relate to social processing, can be detected when TD individuals process upright faces compared to inverted faces or other objects [2022]. In the autistic population, there is accumulating evidence regarding deficits in alpha, beta, and gamma bands’ power and connectivity patterns during social perception [2330]. Mainly, it has been shown that during the evaluation of social stimuli, autistic individuals tend to present under-connectivity in the low-frequency bands, and over-connectivity in the gamma band, especially in magnetoencephalography (MEG) studies [31].

However, the existing literature regarding oscillatory connectivity in ASD during social perception suffers from both inconsistencies and gaps. First, despite extensive research on high-band frequencies during social perception in ASD, the results are not consistent across the literature. While some studies suggest that deficits in gamma activation and synchronization can serve as biomarkers for autism [32], other studies have shown that during face processing, interhemispheric connectivity in gamma can be either increased [3335] or decreased [36, 30]. Moreover, the results are highly dependent on the experimental sample, individuals’ age, chosen task, and the type of the analysis [31, 37]. Second, only a handful of studies examined the phase-lag connectivity of other frequencies, such as alpha and beta-band, which relate more to feedback loops and top-down reasoning. In these frequencies, the results are usually consistent, indicating that compared to TDs, individuals with ASD show hypoconnectivity [38, 23, 39, 40, 36] except some studies showing the opposite direction by reporting an increase in alpha connectivity during social processing [29].

Another scientific approach for investigating the top-down/bottom-up balance in ASD focused on the correlations between low and high frequencies using cross-frequencies methods such as phase–amplitude coupling (PAC) [41]. PAC analysis allows evaluating whether the phase of a low-frequency band (usually alpha) in one region is locked to the amplitude of high-frequency bands in another region [42]. It has been shown that during resting-state measurements, individuals with ASD present increased short-range alpha-to-gamma PAC [43, 44]. During social perception, PAC can shed more light on the balance between top-down and bottom-up perception in ASD by examining the relationships between low and high frequencies. Indeed, studies have shown that individuals with ASD present reduced coupling during face perception in ASD for both long- and short-range connectivity, a reduction that was correlated with clinical symptoms severity [40, 27].

Overall, the literature presents differences in the oscillatory functional connectivity during emotional face processing in ASD, compared to TD. We suggest that these differences may underlie the reported imbalance between top-down and bottom-up facial processing in the autistic population and aim to investigate the influence of Oxytocin (OT) administration on said imbalance.

It has been shown that OT, a naturally occurring hormone that relates to social communication in mammals [45], can influence social perception in autistic individuals. For example, it has been shown that in ASD exogenous OT can modulate face perception, attention toward facial features, and identification abilities of emotional facial expressions [4651]. Although it has been found that the endogenous levels of OT in children are lower than those of TD individuals or adults [52], the influences of exogenous OT are not consistent and depend on the context in which stimuli were presented, individual clinical and psychological parameters, as well as genetic factors such as OT receptor variants or peripheral OT level. Moreover, it is still not clear how OT influences early perceptual stages and whether these possible effects could modulate face perception in ASD.

One possible explanation for the mechanism underlying OT effects, in the context of early perception, emerges from the possible modulation of neural excitation/inhibition (E/I) ratio. The E/I model, developed by Rubenstein and Merzenich [53], proposes that individuals with ASD present higher-than-expected neural activations due to an imbalance between local synaptic signals of excitation and inhibition. Based mainly on animal models of ASD, Lopatina et al. [54] suggest that endogenous and exogenous OT can modify the excitation/inhibition (E/I) ratio through gamma-power modulation and inhibitory GABAergic pathways. Although this theory focused on local circuits, it has been proposed that disruptions in gamma feedforward course can disrupt long-range information transference across brain areas [41, 55]. Thus, it is possible that by modulating the E/I ratio, OT can enhance neural connectivity and improve social perception abilities among ASD. While several studies have shown that OT indeed influences social perception and social-related neural activity in ASD, most of these studies did not examine oscillatory activity.

The current study aimed to investigate the effects of OT on top-down/bottom-up modulation in youth with ASD during the early phases of face perception. Specifically, using MEG, we measured gamma, alpha, and beta bands connectivity between three specific a priori-defined social-related regions of interest (ROIs): the left and the right fusiform and the medial prefrontal region. These regions specialize in the perception of face and facial expressions [5658] and can be modulated by OT in both TD and ASD individuals [49, 59, 60, 61]. However, the effects of OT in these regions during the early phases of social attention are still unknown. We hypothesized that in adolescents diagnosed with ASD, OT would modulate top-down and bottom-up processing during social perception by elevating alpha- and beta-band connectivity and reducing gamma-band connectivity between the mentioned ROIs.

This is the first time such an experimental paradigm has been implemented with MEG in youth with ASD, to the best of our knowledge. Thus, although it is preliminary, we believe that the current study can offer a more comprehensive understanding of the mechanism by which OT can modulate different levels of early perceptions during face processing among autistic individuals.

Methods

Participants

The study was approved by the Beer Yaacov-Ness Ziona Mental Health Center Ethics Board (Declaration of Helsinki) and was registered as a clinical trial (ID: NCT05096676). Thirty-two adolescents diagnosed with ASD arrived at Bar-Ilan University for two sessions for a randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial. Participants were recruited through social media ads and the Bait Echad ASD clinical centers in Israel of the Association for Children at Risk. All participants met the diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5) criteria for ASD. Also, clinical diagnosis was confirmed using the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2) [62], a clinical assessment that was conducted at the Bait Echad clinical center prior to the experimental meeting. Furthermore, we ensured that none of the participants had a comorbid intellectual impairment (cutoff was an IQ > 80) using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI, [63]).

From the entire sample, two participants were unable to complete the session due to technical issues in the MEG, and six individuals were excluded from the analysis due to a high percentage of artifacts, such as muscle movements (where at least 30% of the trails did not allow sufficient processing due to artifacts). Thus, 24 participants were included in the data analysis.

All participants were males, aged 12–18 years, native Hebrew speakers, and had a normal or corrected-to-normal vision (see Table 1 for demographic details). Before the experiment, participants and their parents underwent a telephone screening interview regarding chronic medical problems, cardiovascular risk factors, CNS disease, other mental illnesses, and the use of prohibited medications (see Additional file 1 for list of approved medications). Participants with intellectual disabilities, impaired vision or hearing, current substance dependence diagnosis, a history of significant head injury or neurological illness, or metallic implants were excluded from the study. At the beginning of each visit, parents were provided an informed consent form, and the participant provided verbal assent.

Table 1 Participant characteristics

All participants were paid 500 NIS for their participation in both sessions.

Procedure

Participants visited the laboratory for two sessions (approximately 1 week apart). In one session, they received OT, and in the other session placebo (PL). The order of the substance administration was random, and the design was double-blind. All substances were intranasal doses of 40 international units of (IU)/mL and prepared by the “Maayan Haim” pharmacy, Israel. Age-dependent doses were adapted to participants’ ages: 13–18 years old adolescents received a dose of 24 IU (3 puffs to each nostril), and younger participants (aged 12 years) received 16 IU.

Forty-five minutes following intranasal administration [47, 64], participants underwent MEG scanning and digital registration of the head position. In each scan, instructions were provided regarding the need to minimize head or body movements as much as possible. We verified that all participants felt as comfortable and relaxed as possible before and during each scan.

Paradigm

During each session, participants performed a social-related emotion judgment task based on the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET-R) [65]. In the task, participants were asked to recognize social (pictures of eyes in a variety of emotional expressions) and non-social (pictures of vehicles) grayscale images. Overall, we presented 160 stimuli–20 different images in each category with four repetitions of each set. The order of the blocks and the order of the pictures inside each block were randomized and counterbalanced across participants. The task lasted approximately 13 min in total (see Fig. 1).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Behavioral paradigm. The task consisted of eight blocks—half contained social stimuli (pictures of eyes during emotional expression, see example above the line) and the other half contained non-social stimuli (pictures of vehicles, see example below the line). Trials began with a fixation cross (500 ms). Next, an image appeared for approximately one second, followed by a single word. Participants were asked to decide whether the word described the image. The three dots at the end of each row mark the continuity of the block

In the MEG, participants were placed in a supine position, and stimuli were presented reflected on a mirror from a 17″ screen located 60 cm above their heads using e-Prime software (version 2.0 professional, Psychological Software Tools, USA).

Data analysis

Behavior analysis

The influence of OT on behavioral performance was assessed using repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA), reported with a Bonferroni correction for multiple tests. We conducted a 2 * 2 (OT/PL X social/non-social) design for both accuracy rates and reaction times (RTs, from correct trials only). Finally, we correlated individual parameters (age, WASI, and ADOS) with the effect of OT on each condition.

MEG analysis

Due to the different roles that different bands have in face processing and top-down/bottom-up perception, to investigate the influence of OT on connectivity during social perception we conducted connectivity analyses, examining separately long-distance connectivity patterns in alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (14–25 Hz), and gamma (30–100 Hz) bands. Similar to Sun et al. [35], we split the range of gamma frequencies to low- (30–60 Hz) and high-gamma (60–100 Hz) bands. Furthermore, we focused on specific regions of interest (ROI) in all analyses: the left and right fusiform and the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC, left and right combined). These ROIs were chosen due to their crucial role in face processing and their sensitivity to OT during social-related tasks [49, 56, 59, 50, 57, 58, 61].

Preprocessing

Data analysis was conducted using the Fieldtrip toolbox for MATLAB [66] except for the removal of heartbeat artifacts, line power (50 Hz and its harmonics), and excessive external noise, which used in-lab algorithms [67]. Next, data were segmented into 1400 ms epochs: 400 ms before the appearance of the picture and 1000 ms after. We first partly removed bad sensors which presented irregular electrical conductance. Then, by visual inspection, we removed segments containing muscle artifacts after applying a high-pass filter at 60 Hz. Independent component analysis (ICA) was applied (after downsampling data to 300 Hz) to remove eye blinks, eye movements, and remaining heartbeats. Finally, all segments were filtered in the 1–100 Hz range with 10 s of padding. A trial-by-trial visual inspection was conducted to reject unusual trials. High and low bands were inspected separately.

The final data for each individual included only artifact-free trials in which correct answers were received by the participant (OTsocial: M = 66.6, SD = 7.37; OTnon-social: M = 63.9, SD = 7.86; PLsocial: M = 65.2, SD = 7.42; PLnon-social: M = 64.8, SD = 6.95).

In light of the significant impact that the number of trails has on connectivity measures [68], we equated the number of clean trails for every condition and session by randomly sub-sampling trials according to the participant with the minimal number of trials (in both OT and PL session: nSocial = 34 trials, nnon-Social = 34 trials).

Connectivity analysis

To evaluate the influence of OT on top-down and bottom-up connections, we assess the connectivity in alpha and gamma bands between the a priori selected ROIs. Long-range connectivity in alpha, beta, and gamma ranges was conducted to determine the connectivity between related social regions. We evaluated the medial frontal regions and the left and right fusiform as one network and examined the overall connectivity in it. First, we conducted a time lock analysis on the preprocessed data for each frequency range (alpha: 8–13 Hz; beta: 14–25 Hz; low gamma: 30–60 Hz; and high gamma: 60–100 Hz). We applied a linearly constrained minimum-variance (LCMV) beamformer to the data, time-locked to the appearance of the stimuli, and computed the overall filter for the source model. Next, to take individual differences in brain topography into consideration, we independently choose the voxel with maximal differences between social and non-social trials for each participant. We then evaluate the neural activity using a virtual channel constructed from the maximum power orientation in each chosen voxel and trial. This was conducted for the entire period in which the stimulus appeared (1000 ms) and was corrected for baseline activity (400 ms before stimulus appearance). Finally, we calculated the spectral connectivity between the ROIs for each frequency range by computing the coherence matrix across the vertices and the averaged coherence score (mean score of upper triangular in that metric). This analysis was conducted twice, once for OT sessions and once for the PL sessions. Furthermore, we used a permutation test (10,000 permutations on the group affiliation—OT/PL) to evaluate the influences of OT in social compared to non-social trials. For each frequency band and condition, we computed the mean coherence score between the fusiform (left and right clusters) and the mPFC. The statistic calculated was the difference between the scores obtained for the OT and PL sessions, for which a null distribution (under the hypothesis that there is no difference in coherence between the sessions) was computed as follows: We resampled the data by conducting a per-subject permutation test on the coherence scores. We swapped the labels of the OT and PL sessions with a probability of 0.5. In each iteration, the coherence score was calculated in the same way as the statistic measurement. For the interaction effect, we normalized, in each session, the network’s coherence score by dividing the score from the social trials from that of the non-social trials. The main effect of OT was examined by averaging the coherence score of social and non-social trials in each session and comparing the average scores. All results in the connectivity analysis (8 p values) were false discovery rate (FDR) corrected [69].

In light of the great heterogeneity in the autistic population, we also explored whether OT benefits more with specific individuals by correlating the normalized neural data and individuals’ age and clinical assessment (ADOS and WASI scores).

Brain–behavior analysis

To investigate the unique effects of OT on social perception as observed in the interaction that was revealed in the gamma band, we conducted a brain–behavior exploratory analysis. We calculated a new normalized “social perception score” for each individual. Similar to the connectivity analysis, the “social perception score” was composed of the accuracy rates in the social trials divided by the rates in the non-social trials. This score was calculated for the OT session only. We next examined whether the connectivity in gamma bands correlated with the behavioral measurement. For this correlation analysis, we used normalized coherence and behavioral scores.

Results

Behavioral analysis

Repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to assess the effects of OT on social perception for both reaction times (RTs) and accuracy rates. Reported RTs analysis included only trials with correct responses (social: M = 62, SD = 5.69; non-social: M = 66.6, SD = 4.75).

RT analysis revealed a significant difference between conditions (F(1,22) = 10.16, p < 0.005), as RTs in non-social trials were shorter than those in social trials (t = 3.19, p < 0.005). No significant effects were found for group (F(1,22) = 0.03, p = 0.86) or interaction (F(1,22) = 0.17, p = 0.68). For accuracy rates, no significant effects were found for conditions (F(1,22) = 0.36, p = 0.55), group (F(1,22) = 1.3, p = 0.26), or interaction (F(1,22) = 1.03, p = 0.32). All the reported results are corrected for Bonferroni correction. For descriptive and inferential statistics, see Tables 2 and 3.

Table 2 Reaction time differences between the experimental conditions and sessions
Table 3 Accuracy rate differences between the experimental conditions and sessions

However, by correlating the ADOS score with the normalized accuracy rates (social/non-social score) we found a positive correlation between the influence of OT and individual ADOS scores: In individuals with higher ADOS scores, OT increased the differences between social and non-social perception, such that higher accuracy rates were observed in the social trials (compared to PL sessions) (r = 0.42, p = 0.04). No correlation was found between behavioral performance and WASI scores (r = − 0.05, p = 0.81).

Connectivity analysis

Using the permutation test, we explored the difference in the mean coherence scores of the task conditions between the OT and the PL sessions (see Table 4 for descriptive data of the coherence scores in each band and condition and Table 5 for descriptive and inferential statistics).

Table 4 Coherence scores from the connectivity analysis for each condition and session
Table 5 Differences in mean coherence scores between OT ad PL sessions in each frequency band

In the alpha band, a main effect was observed for OT, such that higher alpha coherence between ROIs was observed after OT administration (uncorrected p = 0.012, qFDR = 0.043, 10,000 permutations). The interaction effect was not significant (uncorrected p = 0.5, 10,000 permutations).

Similar to the alpha band, in the beta range, a main effect was observed for OT, such that higher beta coherence between ROIs was observed for OT, compared to PL (uncorrected p = 0.016, qFDR = 0.043, 10,000 permutations), while the interaction effect was not significant (uncorrected p = 0.27, 10,000 permutations).

However, in gamma band the interaction effect was significant in low (uncorrected p = 0.01, qFDR = 0.0427, 10,000 permutations) and marginally significant for high gamma (uncorrected p = 0.02, qFDR = 0.08, 10,000 permutations). As can be seen in Fig. 2, the coherence in gamma was lower after OT administration, a reduction that was larger in the social, compared to the non-social trials (SocialOT-PL diff =  − 0.02; Non-SocialOT-PL diff =  − 0.006). No significant main effects for OT were observed for low or high gamma (low: uncorrected p = 0.17; high: uncorrected p = 0.15, 10,000 permutations).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Violin plots of the connectivity between posterior and frontal social-related regions in alpha, beta, and gamma frequencies. A Selected regions of interest. Three neural regions were chosen prior to the experiment: left fusiform, right fusiform, and medial frontal cortex. All areas were marked based on AAL atlas’ locations. Black dots represent the selected voxels in each region. For connectivity analysis, we used one voxel from each region. Power analysis was calculated from all the voxels. BE The neural coherence score between ROIs, in each frequency band: B alpha (8–13 Hz); C beta (14–25 Hz); D low gamma (30–60 Hz); and E high gamma (60–100 Hz). OT and PL sessions are represented as separate lines. Asterisks represent a significant result (p < .05) after FDR correction. Dots represent individual data. Black horizontal lines represent the mean. Vertical lines represent mean values \(\pm 1\) SE

Brain–behavior correlation

To evaluate the correlation between the unique neural influence of OT during social processing and the behavioral performance, we conducted an exploratory correlation analysis. We investigate the correlation between the strength of the coherence in the low-gamma band, where we found a differential effect of OT during social versus non-social perception, and the accuracy rates in the task. Namely, we correlated the coherence score to the ratio of change in the behavioral performance between social and non-social stimuli by dividing the accuracy rates from the social trials by those without the social stimulus. A negative correlation was observed between the strength of the low-gamma connectivity and individual accuracy rates (r =  − 0.475, p = 0.02): Low-gamma coherence between the fusiform and the mPFC correlated with better performance in social compared to the non-social trials (see Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Brain–behavior correlation. A significant negative correlation was observed between low-gamma-band coherence and participants’ performance. The X-axis represents normalized coherence—the connectivity between the fusiform and mPFC in the social trials divided by the connectivity in the non-social trials. Y-axis represents normalized behavioral performance—accuracy rates in social trials divided by the rates in the non-social trials

Discussion

The current study examined the effects of a single administration of OT on neural oscillations in adolescents diagnosed with ASD in order to assess its impact on top-down and bottom-up processing in this population. Specifically, we examined the influence of OT on the neural coherence of alpha, beta, and gamma bands during face perception in three ROIs: left and right fusiform and mPFC.

Our results show that OT modified social-related posterior-frontal connectivity in two opposite patterns which depend on the examined frequency band. In the lower range of frequency bands, alpha and beta, OT (compared to placebo) increased the connectivity for both social and non-social stimuli. However, in low gamma, OT induced the opposite effect by causing a decrease in the posterior-frontal connectivity. Moreover, compared to the non-selective increase that has been shown in alpha and beta, in gamma the effect of OT was larger during social trials than during non-social ones. We further show that the decrease in the low-gamma range connectivity, following OT administration, correlated negatively with the success of the individuals in social trials compared to non-social trials. Namely, we show that the lower the connectivity, the higher the ability of the participant to identify social stimuli compared to non-social ones.

One possible explanation regarding the results stems from top-down and bottom-up perspectives. Top-down modulation is characterized by feedback loops from frontal and parietal regions and is thought to be manifested in alpha and beta oscillations [70, 71]. Gamma, on the other hand, is known to reflect bottom-up perceptual processes by passing information in a feedforward manner up through the visual stream [72, 17, 73]. Our results show that a single dose of OT can modulate the balance between bottom-up and top-down processing in ASD for both social and non-social visual stimuli. Several studies have shown that during face processing, autistic individuals tend to process facial expressions based on the local features instead of processing first the global form, a more common strategy in TD individuals [74, 4, 5]. In other words, when looking at facial stimuli autistic individuals tend to give more weight to the bottom-up configural processing over top-down information [75]. According to our results, a possible mechanism for the beneficial effects of OT is its ability to shift the balance from bottom-up processing toward a more top-down-oriented perception. This shift, which occurs, to some extent, in both social and non-social stimuli might be more relevant in facial processing where gestalt integration is preferred. During the presence of a face that requires emotional interpretation, it is possible that the reduction of gamma connectivity, following OT administration, decreased the cognitive weight of the bottom-up information transference from the local features and, together with the overall increase in top-down related connectivity, allowed for better processing of the presented information.

The correlation between low-gamma connectivity and improved social perception in the OT trials can be explained also in terms of the excitation-inhibition (E/I) model, where the signal-to-noise neural ratio is manifested in the activity of gamma and its relationship to alpha power [44, 76]. Shifts in this ratio toward a noisy neural transference may underpin ASD deficits during social and emotional processing [29, 77, 78]. Our results suggest a new perspective for interoperate OT effects, suggesting that during visual perception, exogenous OT strengthens the neural ability for posterior-frontal adaptive information transference by reducing the signal-to-noise ratio. This interpretation corresponds to Lopatina et al. (2018) theoretical paper suggesting that OT influences E/I balance by modulating gamma-band activity. We show here that OT not only modulates gamma band but also influences alpha and beta frequencies which had a crucial role in visual attention. At the same time, although it has been shown that long-distance synchronization in low gamma (around 40 Hz) occurs during face processing [79, 55], gamma connectivity as a “neural binding” mechanism is mostly discussed at the local/synaptic level [80]. Thus, although our results present an important modulation of low and high coherence simultaneously, to establish the influence of OT on the E/I ratio in ASD at a more global level, future studies should examine the relationship between gamma and lower frequencies using cross-frequency methods such as phase–amplitude coupling [41]. We treat the present study as a preliminary study and encourage further studies to explore the specific effects of OT on high-frequency bands during social interpretation more thoroughly. Moreover, it should be remembered that this proposed model is theoretical, and further studies which allow direct measurement of excitatory and inhibitory intracellular processes are required to confirm our hypothesis regarding the relationship between OT effects and the E/I model.

In recent years, there is a debate in the literature regarding the prolonged influences of OT in individuals with ASD. While former studies show that some individuals with ASD can benefit from the administration of OT, in their comprehensive meta-analysis Sikich and colleagues [81] suggest that over time the effects of OT on common social behaviors are not significant. The current study on the other hand highlights the acute influence of OT in adolescents with ASD, Namely, in line with Geschwind’s response [82] to the mentioned paper, we suggest that acute administration of OT enables a neural basis that, in combination with treatments that provide an opportunity to learn, has the potential to promote the processing of social information during treatment in individuals with ASD. Following Ford and Young’s [83] opinion, which highlights the subtle and context-dependent influences of OT, we suggest that acute administration of OT can open a window of time in which the early perceptual stages will be more global-oriented and top-down information will be more meaningful or available to the individual. Specifically, in our opinion, OT can be a good pairing with cognitive treatments that work on the identification of social cues or social-oriented therapy that focuses more on interpretations and appropriate responses during social situations. Using OT as an augmentation tool in this manner can allow for more effective learning of social stimuli during interventions and reduce the time required to achieve the therapeutic goals. Furthermore, our results suggest that OT increases relevant activity more in those with higher capabilities, as can be seen in the positive correlation between OT effects and the ADOS score. This could offer a path to possible new interventions, that has not always existed, for autistic people with higher social capabilities. 

Limitations

Although our study provides new insights regarding the influence of OT on the early modulation of neural processes between social-related regions in ASD, it has several important limitations. First, our sample was composed of only 24 adolescents. A number of parameters limited the choice of the current sample selection. Namely, we focused on youth (ages 12–18) males only with high functional abilities. Moreover, all individuals were required to come to the laboratory twice (or three times if the clinical diagnosis needed to be renewed). In addition, our paradigm, which consists of the administration of OT, imaging sessions, and performance tasks, has led to long and complex meetings. Although the size of our sample is equivalent to the sample sizes from other studies with similar experimental designs [27, 29], given the complexity of the electrophysiology analyses and the neural variability that exists between individuals, we encourage follow-up studies to examine the effects of OT on early attentional processes on a more extensive and heterogenic sample in terms of gender, age, and clinical characteristics. Our sample size can also explain the lack of correlation between OT effects and the age or clinical assessment of the participants. Thus, another advantage of a larger and more diverse sample is identifying the individual who could benefit the most from OT administration.

Conclusions

The current study present the influences of OT on early neural dynamics and connectivity patterns between the fusiform and medial frontal regions during social and non-social processing in ASD. By showing that OT impacts bottom-up and top-down modulation, in a different direction, immediately after the appearance of stimuli, we suggest a new and refined neural mechanism that can explain the social-related effects of OT in individuals with ASD during the earliest stages of processing. Alongside the possible therapeutic implication for OT in ASD during visual perception, our results also contribute to the growing understanding of the relationship between atypical connectivity in adolescents with ASD and their ability to perceive social and non-social stimuli in the world.

Availability of data and materials

Due to the clinical features and the age of our sample as well as the sizes and nature of the imaging files, all data will be sent upon request. Please send an e-mail to the corresponding author for more details.

Abbreviations

ADOS:

Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

ANOVA:

Analysis of variance

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

E/I :

Excitation/inhibition

EEG:

Electroencephalography

ICA:

Independent component analysis

LCMV:

Linearly constrained minimum variance

MEG:

Magnetoencephalography

MIE:

Mind in the eyes

mPFC:

Medial prefrontal cortex

MRI:

Magnetic resonance imaging

OT:

Oxytocin

PL:

Placebo

RMET:

Reading the mind in the eyes task

ROI/s:

Region/s of interest

TD:

Typically developed

WASI:

Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596.744053.

  2. Dawson G, Webb SJ, McPartland J. Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Dev Neuropsychol. 2005;27(3):403–24. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326942dn2703_6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Webb SJ, Neuhaus E, Faja S. Face perception and learning in autism spectrum disorders. Q J Exp Psychol. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470218.2016.1151059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Happé F, Frith U. The weak coherence account: detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0039-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulières I, Hubert B, Burack J. Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Gur RC, Schroeder L, Turner T, McGrath C, Chan RM, Turetsky BI, Alsop D, Maldjian J, Gur RE. Brain activation during facial emotion processing. Neuroimage. 2002;16(3 I):651–62. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1097.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Kesler-West ML, Andersen AH, Smith CD, Avison MJ, Davis CE, Kryscio RJ, Blonder LX. Neural substrates of facial emotion processing using fMRI. Cogn Brain Res. 2001;11(2):213–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(00)00073-2.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Rossion B, Caldara R, Seghier M, Schuller AM, Lazeyras F, Mayer E. A network of occipito-temporal face-sensitive areas besides the right middle fusiform gyrus is necessary for normal face processing. Brain. 2003;126(11):2381–95. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg241.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bookheimer SY, Wang AT, Scott A, Sigman M, Dapretto M. Frontal contributions to face processing differences in autism: evidence from fMRI of inverted face processing. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2008;14(6):922–32. https://doi.org/10.1017/S135561770808140X.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Kleinhans NM, Richards T, Sterling L, Stegbauer KC, Mahurin R, Johnson LC, Greenson J, Dawson G, Aylward E. Abnormal functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders during face processing. Brain. 2008;131(4):1227–36. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm334.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Monk CS, Weng SJ, Wiggins JL, Kurapati N, Louro HMC, Carrasco M, Maslowsky J, Risi S, Lord C. Neural circuitry of emotional face processing in autism spectrum disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2010;35(2):105–14. https://doi.org/10.1503/jpn.090085.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Vandewouw MM, Choi EJ, Hammill C, Lerch JP, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. Changing faces: dynamic emotional face processing in autism spectrum disorder across childhood and adulthood. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2021;6(8):825–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2020.09.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Moradi A, Mehrinejad SA, Ghadiri M, Rezaei F. Event-related potentials of bottom-up and top-down processing of emotional faces. Basic Clin Neurosci. 2017;8(1):27–36. https://doi.org/10.15412/J.BCN.03080104.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  14. Xiu D, Geiger MJ, Kiaver P. Emotional face expression modulates occipital–frontal effective connectivity during memory formation in a bottom-up fashion. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9(APR):90. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00090.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Herrmann CS, Munk MHJ, Engel AK. Cognitive functions of gamma-band activity: memory match and utilization. Trends Cogn Sci. 2004. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.06.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Von Stein A, Sarnthein J. Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int J Psychophysiol. 2000. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0167-8760(00)00172-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Engel AK, Fries P, Singer W. Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1038/35094565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Singer W, Gray CM. Visual feature integration and the temporal correlation hypothesis. Annu Rev Neurosci. 1995. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ne.18.030195.003011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Tallon-Baudry C, Bertrand O. Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01299-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Başar E, Özgören M, Öniz A, Schmiedt C, Başar-Eroǧlu C. Brain oscillations differentiate the picture of one’s own grandmother. Int J Psychophysiol. 2007. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Güntekin B, Başar E. A review of brain oscillations in perception of faces and emotional pictures. Neuropsychologia. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.03.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Zion-Golumbic E, Golan T, Anaki D, Bentin S. Human face preference in gamma-frequency EEG activity. Neuroimage. 2008. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.10.025.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Catarino A, Andrade A, Churches O, Wagner AP, Baron-Cohen S, Ring H. Task-related functional connectivity in autism spectrum conditions: an EEG study using wavelet transform coherence. Mol Autism. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1186/2040-2392-4-1.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  24. Cook J, Barbalat G, Blakemore SJ. Top-down modulation of the perception of other people in schizophrenia and autism. Front Hum Neurosci. 2012;6(June 2012):1–10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00175.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Khan S, Michmizos K, Tommerdahl M, Ganesan S, Kitzbichler MG, Zetino M, Garel KLA, Herbert MR, Hämäläinen MS, Kenet T. Somatosensory cortex functional connectivity abnormalities in autism show opposite trends, depending on direction and spatial scale. Brain. 2015;138(5):1394–409. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awv043.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  26. Loth E, Gómez JC, Happé F. When seeing depends on knowing: adults with autism spectrum conditions show diminished top-down processes in the visual perception of degraded faces but not degraded objects. Neuropsychologia. 2010;48(5):1227–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.12.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Mamashli F, Kozhemiako N, Khan S, Nunes AS, McGuiggan NM, Losh A, Joseph RM, Ahveninen J, Doesburg SM, Hämäläinen MS, Kenet T. Children with autism spectrum disorder show altered functional connectivity and abnormal maturation trajectories in response to inverted faces. Autism Res. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.2497.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Mennella R, Leung RC, Taylor MJ, Dunkley BT. Disconnection from others in autism is more than just a feeling: whole-brain neural synchrony in adults during implicit processing of emotional faces. Mol Autism. 2017;8(1):1–12. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-017-0123-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Safar K, Wong SM, Leung RC, Dunkley BT, Taylor MJ. Increased functional connectivity during emotional face processing in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. 2018. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2018.00408.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Safar K, Yuk V, Wong SM, Leung RC, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. Emotional face processing in autism spectrum disorder: effects in gamma connectivity. Biol Psychol. 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2019.107774.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. O’Reilly C, Lewis JD, Elsabbagh M. Is functional brain connectivity atypical in autism? A systematic review of EEG and MEG studies. PLoS ONE. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0175870.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Rojas DC, Wilson LB. γ-band abnormalities as markers of autism spectrum disorders. Biomark Med. 2014. https://doi.org/10.2217/bmm.14.15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Grice SJ, Spratling MW, Karmiloff-Smith A, Halit H, Csibra G, De Haan M, Johnson MH. Disordered visual processing and oscillatory brain activity in autism and Williams syndrome. NeuroReport. 2001. https://doi.org/10.1097/00001756-200108280-00021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Naumann S, Senftleben U, Santhosh M, McPartland J, Webb SJ. Neurophysiological correlates of holistic face processing in adolescents with and without autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. 2018. https://doi.org/10.1186/s11689-018-9244-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Sun L, Grützner C, Bölte S, Wibral M, Tozman T, Schlitt S, Poustka F, Singer W, Freitag CM, Uhlhaas PJ. Impaired gamma-band activity during perceptual organization in adults with autism spectrum disorders: evidence for dysfunctional network activity in frontal-posterior cortices. J Neurosci. 2012. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1073-12.2012.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Peiker I, David N, Schneider TR, Nolte G, Schöttle D, Engel AK. Perceptual integration deficits in autism spectrum disorders are associated with reduced interhemispheric gamma-band coherence. J Neurosci. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1442-15.2015.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Safar K, Vandewouw MM, Taylor MJ. Atypical development of emotional face processing networks in autism spectrum disorder from childhood through to adulthood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. 2021;51:101003. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2021.101003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  38. Carson AM, Salowitz NMG, Scheidt RA, Dolan BK, Van Hecke AV. Electroencephalogram coherence in children with and without autism spectrum disorders: decreased interhemispheric connectivity in autism. Autism Res. 2014. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Isler JR, Martien KM, Grieve PG, Stark RI, Herbert MR. Reduced functional connectivity in visual evoked potentials in children with autism spectrum disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Khan S, Gramfort A, Shetty NR, Kitzbichler MG, Ganesan S, Moran JM, Lee SM, Gabrieli JDE, Tager-Flusberg HB, Joseph RM, Herbert MR, Hämäläinen MS, Kenet T. Local and long-range functional connectivity is reduced in concert in autism spectrum disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(8):3107–12. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1214533110.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Kessler K, Seymour RA, Rippon G. Brain oscillations and connectivity in autism spectrum disorders (ASD): new approaches to methodology, measurement and modelling. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;71:601–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.10.002.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Canolty RT, Knight RT. The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn Sci. 2010;14(11):506–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  43. Berman JI, Liu S, Bloy L, Blaskey L, Roberts TPL, Edgar JC. Alpha-to-gamma phase-amplitude coupling methods and application to autism spectrum disorder. Brain Connect. 2015;5(2):80–90. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2014.0242.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  44. Port RG, Dipiero MA, Ku M, Liu S, Blaskey L, Kuschner ES, Edgar JC, Roberts TPL, Berman JI. Children with autism spectrum disorder demonstrate regionally specific altered resting-state phase-amplitude coupling. Brain Connect. 2019;9(5):425–36. https://doi.org/10.1089/brain.2018.0653.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Gimpl G, Fahrenholz F. The oxytocin receptor system: structure, function, and regulation. Physiol Rev. 2001;81(2):629-83. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.2001.81.2.629.

  46. Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, Heinrichs M, Chakrabarti B, Sule A, Deakin JB, Bethlehem RAI, Dickens L, Mooney N, Sipple JAN, Thiemann P, Baron-Cohen S. Oxytocin increases eye contact during a real-time, naturalistic social interaction in males with and without autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2015. https://doi.org/10.1038/tp.2014.146.

  47. Guastella AJ, Einfeld SL, Gray KM, Rinehart NJ, Tonge BJ, Lambert TJ, Hickie IB. Intranasal oxytocin improves emotion recognition for youth with autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2010;67(7):692–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.020.

  48. Andari E, Richard N, Leboyer M, Sirigu A. Adaptive coding of the value of social cues with oxytocin, an fMRI study in autism spectrum disorder. Cortex/ 2016;76:79–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.12.010

  49. Gordon I, Vander Wyk BC, Bennett RH, Cordeaux C, Lucas MV, Eilbott JA, Zagoory-Sharon O, Leckman JF, Feldman R, Pelphrey KA. Oxytocin enhances brain function in children with autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(52):20953–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1312857110.

  50. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Kumbier E, Grossmann A, Hauenstein K, Herpertz SC. Effects of intranasal oxytocin on the neural basis of face processing in autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;74(3):164–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.02.007.

  51. Uzefovsky F, Bethlehem RAI, Shamay-Tsoory S, Ruigrok A, Holt R, Spencer M, Chura L, Warrier V, Chakrabarti B, Bullmore E, Suckling J, Floris D, Baron-Cohen S. The oxytocin receptor gene predicts brain activity during an emotion recognition task in autism. Molecular Autism. 2019. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-019-0258-4.

  52. Moerkerke M, Peeters M, de Vries L, Daniels N, Steyaert J, Alaerts K, Boets B. Endogenous oxytocin levels in autism—a meta-analysis. Brain Sci. 2021;11(11):1545. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci11111545.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. 2003;2(5):255-67.‏

  54. Lopatina OL, Komleva YK, Gorina YV, Olovyannikova RY, Trufanova LV, Hashimoto T, Takahashi T, Kikuchi M, Minabe Y, Higashida H, Salmina AB. Oxytocin and excitation/inhibition balance in social recognition. Neuropeptides. 2018;72:1-11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.npep.2018.09.003.

  55. Varela F, Lachaux JP, Rodriguez E, Martinerie J. The brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2001;2(4):229–39. https://doi.org/10.1038/35067550.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Grill-Spector K, Knouf N, Kanwisher N. The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nat Neurosci. 2004;7(5):555–62. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1224.

  57. Etkin A, Egner T, Kalisch R. Emotional processing in anterior cingulate and medial prefrontal cortex. Trends Cogn Sci. 2011;15(2):85–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.11.004.

  58. Kanwisher N, Yovel G. The fusiform face area: a cortical region specialized for the perception of faces. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2006;361(1476):2109–28. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1934.

  59. Domes G, Heinrichs M, Michel A, Berger C, Herpertz SC. Oxytocin improves "mind-reading" in humans. Biol Psychiatry. 2007;61(6):731-3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.07.015.

  60. Kanat M, Heinrichs M, Domes G. Oxytocin and the social brain: neural mechanisms and perspectives in human research. Brain Res. 2014;1580:160–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.003.

  61. Petrovic P, Kalisch R, Singer T, Dolan RJ. Oxytocin attenuates affective evaluations of conditioned faces and amygdala activity. J Neurosci. 2008;28(26):6607-15. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4572-07.2008.

  62. Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH Jr, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, Rutter M. The autism diagnostic observation schedule-generic: a standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. J Autism Dev Disord. 2000;30(3):205–23.

  63. Wechsler D. Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence—fourth edition. The Psychological Corporation San Antonio, TX.‏ 2012.

  64. Hollander E, Novotny S, Hanratty M, Yaffe R, DeCaria CM, Aronowitz BR, Mosovich S. Oxytocin infusion reduces repetitive behaviors in adults with autistic and Asperger's disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2003;28(1):193-8. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.npp.1300021.

  65. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The "Reading the Mind in the Eyes" Test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2001;42(2):241–51. https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00715.

  66. Oostenveld R, Fries P, Maris E, Schoffelen JM. FieldTrip: Open source software for advanced analysis of MEG, EEG, and invasive electrophysiological data. Comput Intell Neurosci. 2011;2011:156869. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/156869.

  67. Tal I, Abeles M. Cleaning MEG artifacts using external cues. J Neurosci Methods. 2013. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.04.002.

  68. Bastos AM, Schoffelen JM. A Tutorial Review of Functional Connectivity Analysis Methods and Their Interpretational Pitfalls. Front Syst Neurosci. 2016;9:175. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnsys.2015.00175.

  69. Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the false discovery rate: a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. J Royal Stat Society Ser B (Methodol). 1995;57(1):289–300.‏

  70. Bonnefond M, Jensen O. Gamma activity coupled to alpha phase as a mechanism for top-down controlled gating. PLoS One. 2015;10(6):e0128667. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0128667.

  71. Klimesch W. Alpha-band oscillations, attention, and controlled access to stored information. Trends Cogn Sci. 2012;16(12):606–617. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.10.007.

  72. Bastos AM, Vezoli J, Bosman CA, Schoffelen JM, Oostenveld R, Dowdall JR, DeWeerd P, Kennedy H, Fries P. Visual areas exert feedforward and feedback influences through distinct frequency channels. Neuron. 2015;85(2):390–401. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.018.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Fries P. A mechanism for cognitive dynamics: neuronal communication through neuronal coherence. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(10):474–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2005.08.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Behrmann M, Avidan G, Leonard GL, Kimchi R, Luna B, Humphreys K, Minshew N. Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(1):110–29. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.04.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Neumann D, Spezio ML, Piven J, Adolphs R. Looking you in the mouth: abnormal gaze in autism resulting from impaired top-down modulation of visual attention. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2006;1(3):194–202. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsl030.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  76. Lee E, Lee J, Kim E. Excitation/inhibition imbalance in animal models of autism spectrum disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2017;81(10):838–847.

  77. Cochran DM, Sikoglu EM, Hodge SM, Edden RA, Foley A, Kennedy DN, Moore CM, Frazier JA. Relationship among Glutamine, γ-Aminobutyric Acid, and Social Cognition in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. 2015;25(4):314-22. https://doi.org/10.1089/cap.2014.0112.

  78. Uzunova G, Pallanti S, Hollander E. Excitatory/inhibitory imbalance in autism spectrum disorders: Implications for interventions and therapeutics. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2016;17(3):174-86. https://doi.org/10.3109/15622975.2015.1085597.

  79. Rodriguez E, George N, Lachaux JP, Martinerie J, Renault B, Varela FJ. Perception’s shadow: long-distance synchronization of human brain activity. Nature. 1999;397(6718):430–3. https://doi.org/10.1038/17120.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Brock J, Brown CC, Boucher J, Rippon G. The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Dev Psychopathol. 2002;14(2):209–24. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579402002018.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. Sikich L, Kolevzon A, King BH, McDougle CJ, Sanders KB, Kim SJ, Spanos M, Chandrasekhar T, Trelles MDP, Rockhill CM, Palumbo ML, Witters Cundiff A, Montgomery A, Siper P, Minjarez M, Nowinski LA, Marler S, Shuffrey LC, Alderman C, Weissman J, Zappone B, Mullett JE, Crosson H, Hong N, Siecinski SK, Giamberardino SN, Luo S, She L, Bhapkar M, Dean R, Scheer A, Johnson JL, Gregory SG, Veenstra-VanderWeele J. Intranasal Oxytocin in Children and Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorder. N Engl J Med. 2021;385(16):1462-1473.‏

  82. Geschwind DH. Oxytocin for Autism Spectrum Disorder - Down, but Not Out. N Engl J Med. 2021;385(16):1524–1525. https://doi.org/10.1056/NEJMe2110158.

  83. Ford CL, Young LJ. Refining oxytocin therapy for autism: context is key. Nat Rev Neurol. 2022;18(2):67–8. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41582-021-00602-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

A special thanks to all the adolescents and their families who participated in this study.

Funding

Abraham Goldstein is funded by the Israeli Science Foundation (ISF Grant #726/15). Ilanit Gordon is funded by the Israeli Science Foundation (ISF Grants #1726/15; #2096/15).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

AK contributed to conceptualization; data curation; formal analysis; writing—original draft; and visualization. AG was involved in conceptualization; formal analysis; funding acquisition; supervision; resources; and writing—review and editing and provided software. IG contributed to conceptualization; funding acquisition; investigation; resources; supervision; and writing—review and editing. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ilanit Gordon.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This study was carried out following the recommendations of the medical ethics committee of Israel and was approved by the Beer-Yaacov-Ness-Ziona Mental Health Center Ethics Board (#537) in accordance with the Helsinki declaration. Before the beginning of the study’s sessions, participants' parents provided informed consent.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests and no competing personal or financial interests that could influence the study in this paper.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Medication information.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Korisky, A., Gordon, I. & Goldstein, A. Oxytocin impacts top-down and bottom-up social perception in adolescents with ASD: a MEG study of neural connectivity. Molecular Autism 13, 36 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00513-6

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-022-00513-6

Keywords