Skip to main content

How rare and common risk variation jointly affect liability for autism spectrum disorder

Abstract

Background

Genetic studies have implicated rare and common variations in liability for autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Of the discovered risk variants, those rare in the population invariably have large impact on liability, while common variants have small effects. Yet, collectively, common risk variants account for the majority of population-level variability. How these rare and common risk variants jointly affect liability for individuals requires further study.

Methods

To explore how common and rare variants jointly affect liability, we assessed two cohorts of ASD families characterized for rare and common genetic variations (Simons Simplex Collection and Population-Based Autism Genetics and Environment Study). We analyzed data from 3011 affected subjects, as well as two cohorts of unaffected individuals characterized for common genetic variation: 3011 subjects matched for ancestry to ASD subjects and 11,950 subjects for estimating allele frequencies. We used genetic scores, which assessed the relative burden of common genetic variation affecting risk of ASD (henceforth “burden”), and determined how this burden was distributed among three subpopulations: ASD subjects who carry a potentially damaging variant implicated in risk of ASD (“PDV carriers”); ASD subjects who do not (“non-carriers”); and unaffected subjects who are assumed to be non-carriers.

Results

Burden harbored by ASD subjects is stochastically greater than that harbored by control subjects. For PDV carriers, their average burden is intermediate between non-carrier ASD and control subjects. Both carrier and non-carrier ASD subjects have greater burden, on average, than control subjects. The effects of common and rare variants likely combine additively to determine individual-level liability.

Limitations

Only 305 ASD subjects were known PDV carriers. This relatively small subpopulation limits this study to characterizing general patterns of burden, as opposed to effects of specific PDVs or genes. Also, a small fraction of subjects that are categorized as non-carriers could be PDV carriers.

Conclusions

Liability arising from common and rare risk variations likely combines additively to determine risk of any individual diagnosed with ASD. On average, ASD subjects carry a substantial burden of common risk variation, even if they also carry a rare PDV affecting risk.

Background

The genetic architecture of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) remains uncertain, although there are putative models [1]. One model posits that ASD is heterogeneous (heterogeneity model), that severe mutations in any of a large set of genes are sufficient to cause the disorder. Another emphasizes the major role played by common variation—shared by all of us to a greater or lesser extent (infinitesimal model)—in the documented high heritability of ASD [2,3,4,5,6]. A hybrid model asserts that common and rare variations combine in some way, perhaps additively [7], to confer liability [1, 7, 8]. At the level of a population, common variation probably plays the dominant role in liability, whereas a rare mutation can make the largest contribution to liability for an individual who carries it [9]. Still, our current understanding of the genetic architecture of ASD is unsatisfactory, especially regarding how common and rare variations jointly confer risk. This architecture is important because it has clinical consequences; for example, it could require more nuanced evaluation of recurrence risk. Establishing the exact nature of the interplay between common and rare risk variations will be challenging, however, because of the multiplicity of plausible models that could fit the current data.

The infinitesimal and additive models differ largely in the magnitude of variants’ impact on liability. Furthermore, because a strict infinitesimal model does not fit the empirical ASD data well, due to documented large effects from some rare variants, we only consider the additive model here. Notably, while heterogeneity and additive models are fundamentally different, they can share key elements, as a recent study by Oetjens and colleagues [10] describes. For subjects carrying mutations for one of 11 rare genetic disorders, Oetjens and colleagues show that quantitative traits associated with these disorders vary substantially as a function of the common genetic variation they carry and they speculate that these rare and common variants could act additively to affect the traits. Yet, they note that it would be hard to distinguish additive from non-additive models even for these quantitative traits without large data sets (see also [11, 12]). In the context of ASD and its binary diagnosis, distinguishing heterogeneity and additive models will be even more challenging.

To address this problem empirically, a sample of ASD subjects who have been characterized for rare and common variations is essential. Because rare, de novo potentially damaging mutations, henceforth PDVs, carry the most readily detectable signal for ASD association [13,14,15,16], the ideal sample would be characterized for such PDVs. Following the tradition in human genetics, we call ASD subjects carrying such PDVs as “PDV carriers” and all other ASD subjects as “non-carriers.” Such well-characterized samples of the population are not common and none as yet are especially large, a limiting factor for any study. For the sample to be analyzed here, we combine data from three sources: subjects diagnosed with ASD from the Simons Simplex Collection or SSC [17]; ASD and unaffected subjects from the Population-Based Autism Genetics and Environment Study or PAGES [9]; and subjects from the Electronic MEdical Records and Genomics Network or eMERGE [18], whom we assume have not been diagnosed with ASD and are non-carriers.

These data could be analyzed in at least two ways. One approach would be to use polygenic risk scores (PRS), which are based on common variants putatively affecting liability [19]. Typically, these variants are identified from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) and the PRS for each subject is computed as a weighted sum of the count of risk alleles they carry. Then, values of the PRS in ASD PDV carriers (ASD-PDV) and ASD non-carriers (ASD-NO-PDV), as well as unaffected subjects, can be contrasted to assess how common and rare variations jointly confer risk. An elegant version of this approach is the pTDT or polygenic Transmission Disequilibrium Test [7], which requires parental genotypes. Because only a portion of our data have parental genotypes, here we concentrate on the PRS.

The PRS is only as effective as the information arising from GWAS, which for ASD is still relatively limited compared to other phenotypes (see Grove and colleagues [6] versus the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium for schizophrenia and bipolar disorder [20, 21]). For this reason, we emphasize here another approach to developing a score, which will be based on the theory of Genomic-Best Linear Unbiased Prediction (G-BLUP) [22,23,24]. The ideas behind G-BLUP are similar to the PRS. Rather than using GWAS results, G-BLUP develops a predictive model to distinguish case versus control subjects, genetically, using genetic variation across the genome (see Additional File 1 for more details on G-BLUP). Here, we call this genomic prediction “GP.” If effective, the PRS and GP will not be strictly independent. Yet, if they were not strongly dependent, they could be combined to produce an even more effective predictor. To allow for this possibility, we tune GP to the population samples used in this study, whereas we use PRS based on different samples. Moreover, because we use a pruning and thresholding approach to the PRS, we chose a threshold that forced the number of SNPs included in the score to be relatively sparse, yet informative, and thereby limiting its correlation with GP. Note that our purpose here is not to compare the predictions of GP and PRS, the former tuned to the population sample and the latter not, but rather to develop predictors useful for examining how common and rare variations jointly confer risk of ASD.

We use these approaches to document (1) that the burden of risk variants carried by ASD subjects is stochastically greater than that carried by control subjects; (2) that carriers of rare, PDVs bear a burden intermediate between non-carrier and control subjects; (3) that both PDV carriers and non-carriers have a stochastically greater burden of common risk variation than control subjects; and (4) that the effects of common and rare variants on liability for ASD likely combine additively. Regarding (3), it appears that ASD subjects carry a substantial burden of common risk variation, even if they also carry a rare PDV affecting risk. For (4), although common and rare risk variations likely act additively, the resolution imposed by the current data is coarse.

Methods and results

Data

Here, we present analyses of 17,972 samples of European descent (Additional file 1: Fig. S1): 3011 were diagnosed with ASD, while the remaining 14,961 were assumed to be unaffected. Among the ASD subjects, 1996 and 1015 came from SSC and PAGES, respectively. Control subjects came from the PAGES (1524) and eMERGE (13,437) cohorts. All subjects from the SSC and eMERGE were collected in the USA; subjects for PAGES come from Sweden. DNA from all subjects was genotyped on an Illumina genotyping platform: Human1M_v1, Human1M_Duov3, and HumanOmni-2.5 for SSC; Infinium OmniExpressExome-8 V1, Infinium OmniExpressExome-8 V1.1-V1.4, and Infinium Expanded Multi-Ethnic Genotyping Array for PAGES; and Illumina Human660W_Quad_v1_A for eMERGE (Additional file 1: Table S1). Genotypes for all samples were imputed at the Michigan Imputation Server [25] using the HRC reference panel [26]. Post-imputation quality control reduced the number of SNPs to 5,145,175. SNPs in high linkage disequilibrium (LD) were thinned using PLINK 2.0 [27] (--clump-r2 0.81; --clump-kb 50), producing 910,356 SNPs, and SNPs that were physically genotyped in more subsets were given preference in the thinning process. A final pruning step with parameters r2 < 0.64 for moving blocks of 50 SNPs and a window of 5 SNPs at a time reduced the genotyped dataset to 553,406 SNPs to be used for all GP calculations.

Table 1 Distribution of the subjects over the four ancestry clusters by ASD status and cohort

Genetic ancestry

To assess ancestry, 99,509 SNPs were selected because they were genotyped for all samples and were relatively independent (the larger set of SNPs was pruned to reduce linkage disequilibrium, as measured by pairwise r2: 50 SNP moving blocks, 5 SNP at a time, pairwise r2 < 0.64). Using genotypes from these SNPs, genetic ancestry was determined using function “clusterGem” in the package GemTools [28]. All 17,972 samples were clustered simultaneously using three ancestry eigenvectors, which divided the sample into four clusters (Table 1; Additional file S1: Fig. S2). Within each cluster, genetically matched pairs were chosen using the function “pairmatch” in library optmatch in R (1-to-1 fullmatch), which assessed pairwise distances among subjects based on the space defined by three ancestry eigenvectors (Additional file 1: Figs. S2–S3). The pairwise distances between the subjects of the matched pairs were far smaller than the average distance for all possible pairs (Additional file 1: Fig. S2).

Approach to estimating G-BLUP

Overview

Our goal in this section is to employ a G-BLUP estimation procedure for GP that distinguishes ASD from unaffected subjects and is free of the influence of ancestry (see Additional file 1 for review of G-BLUP). Note that G-BLUP relies on genetically estimated relationships among subjects in a sample. To calculate this genomic relationship matrix (GRM), genotypes are standardized according to allele frequencies of the SNPs [29, 30]. Data from multiple subpopulations can be challenging if the number of subjects per subpopulation is unbalanced because allele frequency estimates are dominated by the largest subpopulation(s). As a result, the GRM is well estimated within the main subpopulation, but relationship estimates for subjects in other subpopulations will tend to be biased (Additional file 1: Tables S2–S3, Fig. S4). To overcome this concern, we divide our data into ancestry clusters; determine allele frequencies within clusters; standardize the genotypes within clusters; and then calculate the GRM based on these cluster-specific standardizations. This is not the sole concern regarding experimental design. If the ASD and unaffected subjects are not balanced over the ancestry space, bias can be induced in estimation of GP. Specifically, this imbalance can produce misleading differentiation between ASD and unaffected subjects. For this reason, we estimate GP based on a set of ancestry-matched case–control samples [31], thereby avoiding the problem of unbalanced sampling. See Supplemental Methods for a more detailed description of G-BLUP.

Genomic relationship matrix

Using unaffected subjects not matched to ASD subjects, we obtained within-cluster and overall allele frequency estimates. Then, from these two estimates, we used empirical Bayes methods, as described in Bodea et al. [32], to determine final cluster-specific allele-frequency estimates. These frequencies were then used to standardize cluster-specific genotypes and compute a cluster-specific GRM. We call this the cluster-specific GRM, or CLS-GRM, to differentiate it from a GRM computed from genotypes standardized by the mean allele frequency for each SNP using all 11,950 unmatched controls, which we call POP-GRM. Finally, we computed a GRM using the default approach implemented in GCTA software [29, 30], which uses all 17,972 samples (GCTA-GRM). We also used the GCTA software to calculate the first ten principal components of ancestry.

Approach to G-BLUP estimation of GP

For GP to discriminate ASD from unaffected subjects, the expected number of risk alleles in ASD subjects should be stochastically greater than that in unaffected subjects. As the difference in the relative number of risk alleles, which we call the “burden,” becomes greater between the two subpopulations, the GP becomes more accurate. A common way to build a GP is to split the data into a training set and a test set. For the training set, diagnosis is known and is used to develop the model for GP, which is then evaluated in the test set. A common breakdown is to use 90% of the sample for training, which can be done repeatedly using different portions of the sample. An expectation of statistical theory is that a larger training set yields a more accurate GP. For this reason, we chose a training and testing plan with N − 2 observations for training and a matched pair for testing. This is iterated over all matched possible pairs, making it also a computationally intensive plan. To make the plan feasible, we implemented computing techniques to expedite calculations (Additional file 1).

We analyzed N = 6022 (3011 matched pairs). CLS-GRM was used for the genetic relationship among the matched samples, without additional covariates. G-BLUP calculations require an estimate of heritability for ASD, which we set at 0.70 [33]. GP estimates were standardized to have mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1.

Identifying carriers of PDVs

ASD subjects were classified as PDV carriers if their DNA had a protein truncating variant (PTV) or deleterious missense variant (missense badness, PolyPhen-2, and constraint score, MPC > 2) [34] in one of 102 genes identified in Satterstrom et al. [13] as affecting risk. DNA from ASD subjects from the SSC sample was characterized for de novo PDVs using whole-genome sequence, as reported by An et al. [35]. DNA from PAGES affected subjects was characterized for PTV and MIS variants from whole-exome sequence, as reported in Satterstrom et al. [13]: 778 out of 1015 ASD subjects were analyzed and de novo status was unknown for all PDVs. For the PAGES subjects whose DNA was not characterized, we assumed they were non-carriers.

We also included CNVs as PDVs. For the PAGES sample, we used the set of damaging CNVs described in Mahjani et al. [36], who identified CNVs for 956 out of the 1015 ASD subjects we analyzed. For the SSC sample, CNVs were identified by Sanders et al. [14] and damaging status defined following Mahjani et al. [36]. For each dataset, subjects who carried a trisomy or had large or multiple CNVs were set to “undetermined” for carrier status and thus were not in the PDV carrier versus non-carrier analyses, although they were retained as ASD subjects.

DNA from 305 ASD subjects carried one or more PDVs (Table 2; Additional file 1: Table S4). If a subject carried multiple PDVs, the most severe PDV for each subject was counted, under the assumption that the ranking of severity was CNV > PTV > MIS [14].

Table 2 Counts of most severe potentially damaging variants (CNV > PTV > MIS)

Results for GP

To ensure matching was adequate, we first tested whether GP differed between clusters, after controlling for diagnosis. In this analysis-of-variance model, GP did not differ significantly by cluster (F = 0.795, df = 3, 6017; P = 0.497). The relative risk of being an ASD subject, as opposed to unaffected, increased with GP (logistic regression OR = 1.67; 95% CI 1.58–1.77; P = 6.73 × 10−32; pseudo-R2 = 7.80%). Because GP is continuous and standardized, the increased risk should be interpreted in units of standard deviations of GP. Within ASD subjects, PDV carriers had a significantly smaller burden of common risk variants, on average, than do non-carriers (Fig. 1), which is also reflected in the relative risk as a function of GP (OR = 0.81; 95% CI 0.71–0.92; P = 8.36 × 10−4). Both PDV carriers and non-carrier ASD subjects had a greater average GP than unaffected subjects (Fig. 1). Including three or ten eigenvectors of “ancestry” as covariates in the model did not alter these conclusions (Additional file 1: Tables S5–S6), although adding ten eigenvectors as covariates diminished somewhat the difference in GP between ASD and unaffected subjects. One possible explanation is that eigenvectors can be a function of both the burden of risk variation and ancestry.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The burden of risk variation by ASD status and by carrier status of ASD subjects. Burden is estimated using GP, standardized to have mean = 0 and standard deviation = 1. ASD-NO-PDV ASD subjects with no known potentially damaging variants, ASD-PDV ASD subjects with known potentially damaging variants

Conducting other exploratory analyses, we showed the following: CLS allele frequencies were better for standardizing genotypes than population-level allele frequencies (Additional file 1: Table S7); in some settings, adjusting by eigenvectors of ancestry can overcome the inaccuracy induced by population-level allele frequencies (Additional file 1: Table S8); our training/testing plan of N-2/2, where the two individuals comprise a matched pair, was optimal relative to other N-X/X for X > 2 (Additional file 1: Tables S9–S10), and for data sets imbalanced in affected and unaffected subjects within the genetic ancestry space, such as Table 1, this imbalance biased GP (Additional file 1: Fig. S5). We note that while the training/testing plan of N-2/2 appears optimal, it is also over-fitting the data. Therefore, all significance tests contrasting ASD versus unaffected GP were corrected by genomic control (GC, λ = 2.406) [37]; see Additional file 1: Fig. S6. Estimating GP by cohort shows that both contribute to mean differences between ASD and unaffected individuals (Additional file 1: Table S11). GP is less accurate for the PAGES than for the SSC cohorts, which is consistent with several factors: Ascertainment of the PAGES and SSC cohorts was quite different; PAGES ASD subjects were more likely to be carriers, as judged by possibly damaging CNVs (Table 2); and sample size was larger for the SSC than PAGES cohorts and GP will tend to be tailored to the attributes of the larger sample.

Polygenic risk scores and a weighted score

Approach

We evaluated two PRS, specifically one derived from an ASD GWAS [6] and the other from a recent schizophrenia (SCZ) GWAS [20] using a pruning and thresholding approach (Additional file 1), with the threshold set to include only SNPs with P values < 0.01. As described in “Background”, this threshold is chosen to ensure the PRS would be relatively independent of GP. For the ASD GWAS, we used the results based only on the Danish iPsych data because SSC and a portion of the PAGES data were included in the full GWAS analysis [6]. After quality control (QC), described in Additional file 1 9,983 and 26,972 SNPs were included for ASD and SCZ PRS, respectively.

Results for PRS

For the 3011 matched pairs previously described, both the ASD-PRS and the SCZ-PRS distinguished ASD from unaffected subjects, on average (Table 3; Fig. 2). The ASD-PRS also was significantly different for PDV carrier versus non-carrier ASD subjects, as is the SCZ-PRS (Table 3; Fig. 3). Results for a pTDT analysis for ASD were similar to those for the PRS (Additional file 1: Table S11). We also explored a PRS built from a GWAS for educational attainment: it weakly distinguished ASD from unaffected subjects, but not carrier versus non-carrier status (Additional file 1).

Table 3 Logistic regression of ASD status on G-BLUP and PDV status in cases on G-BLUP, ASD-PRS, SCZ-PRS, and the weighted genomic risk score (WGRS)
Fig. 2
figure 2

Distribution of risk scores divided by ASD and unaffected subjects. Burden is estimated using GP, an ASD polygenic risk score (ASD PRS), a schizophrenia polygenic risk score (SCZ PRS), and a weighted genomic risk score that incorporates information from GP, ASD PRS, and SCZ PRS

Fig. 3
figure 3

Distribution of risk scores divided by ASD PDV carriers and non-carriers. Burden is estimated as noted in Fig. 2, here comparing ASD PDVs carriers (ASD-PDV) to non-carriers (ASD-NO-PDV)

Combining scores

The correlation between GP and ASD-PRS was 0.079 (P = 9.80 × 10−10), while it was 0.099 for GP and SCZ-PRS (P = 1.16 × 10−14). ASD-PRS and SCZ-PRS were stochastically uncorrelated (r = 0.007, P = 0.581). Thus, given their modest correlations, these three scores had essentially independent information. To combine them into one weighted genomic risk score (WGRS), we based the weights on their Nagelkerke’s pseudo-R2, which roughly measures each score’s ability to separate ASD and unaffected subjects (Table 3). The WGRS was standardized to have mean 0 and standard deviation of 1. Compared to the other single scores, WGRS better distinguished ASD from unaffected subjects and carrier/non-carrier status of ASD subjects (Table 3; Figs. 2, 3).

We also explored two alternative weighting schemes for obtaining a WGRS, compared to our a priori approach: 1) equal weights and 2) an optimal weighting scheme based on training/testing of the data, as described in Additional file 1: Table S11. Both led to similar WGRS, compared to our a priori approach, and thus also produced similar results.

Relationship between common and rare risk variants

We next ask about the interplay of common and rare risk variants. While diagnosis of ASD is binary, a person either does or does not meet diagnostic criteria, continuous variability of phenotypes related to ASD has long been recognized. A related mathematical observation is that a continuous liability model, in which a normally distributed liability is determined by genetic and environmental risk factors carried by each subject in a population, can be an excellent model for a binary trait like ASD (Fig. 4). In such a model, a liability threshold t determines whether an individual meets the diagnostic criteria and this threshold maps onto ASD prevalence (Fig. 4). If we take prevalence to be 1.5% for the population [13], it sets the threshold for diagnosis of ASD, in terms of a z-score, establishes the mean liability for ASD and control subjects, and thus defines the difference in average liability between ASD and unaffected subjects (Fig. 4).

Fig. 4
figure 4

Continuous liability model for ASD compared to empirical realization. a Liability is assumed to be normally distributed in the population, and there exists a threshold t of liability beyond which everyone is affected and below which no one is affected. ASD prevalence determines t and the average risk of ASD and unaffected subjects; given an estimate of the relative risk of ASD due to rare damaging variants, the average risk of PDV carriers of these PDVs is also specified. b A portion of A, highlighting the relatively even spacing between its average risks. c The realized average risk of these three groups, as measured by the weighted genomic risk score with their 95% confidence intervals (Fig. 3)

For this prevalence, the average liability for ASD subjects is 2.525 and for unaffected subjects it is slightly below zero, − 0.038 (Fig. 4a). To determine where the average carrier ASD subject would fall on this continuum of liability, we require an estimate of the relative risk due to such PDVs. Using results from Satterstrom et al. [13] and Sanders et al. [14], a relative risk of 15 is a good approximation. We can use standard theory, which is described in Satterstrom et al. [13], and elsewhere, to compute the expected liabilities for unaffected and affected individuals, as well as PDV carrier and non-carrier ASD status, assuming common variants and PDVs combine additively to determine liability. Liability for the average carrier would be 1.277 (Fig. 4a), which falls close to the mid-point between the average liabilities of ASD and unaffected subjects, 1.282 (Fig. 4a, b). How does this compare to results for WGRS (Fig. 4c)? For ASD PDV carriers, the average WGRS is 0.045, close to the midpoint (0.0) between the average WGRS for ASD (0.256) and unaffected subjects (− 0.256). Thus, these calculations suggest that rare damaging risk variants and common risk variation act roughly additively to confer liability to ASD. Moreover, when we evaluated whether these results could simply arise by the way we estimated GP, they could not (Additional file 1).

The number of ASD PDV carriers is too few to evaluate liability much more deeply, at least reliably. If the additive model were a close approximation to reality, we would anticipate that the burden observed in ASD subjects would vary inversely with the relative risk of ASD associated with the damaging rare variants they carry. Estimated relative risks associated with pathogenic CNVs and PTV tend to be similar and large [14], whereas the relative risk associated with missense PDVs tend to be far smaller. When we partition PDV carriers according to the type of variant they harbor and compare their burden to that of non-carriers and unaffected subjects, results are consistent with an additive model (Fig. 5): The average burden for ASD subjects carrying CNVs is smallest, although only slightly smaller than the average burden of those carrying PTV PDVs; as might be expected, the difference between these two groups is not meaningful; whereas the average burden of ASD subjects carrying missense variants is substantially larger. By contrast, one might expect that carriers with PDVs in genes commonly disrupted in ASD subjects, such as CHD8 [38,39,40], would bear a smaller burden, on average, than carriers of PDVs in genes disrupted far less often. A confounder here is gene size, larger ASD risk genes will tend accrue more PDVs than smaller genes. While imperfect, a rough way to account for this confounder is to use the TADA Bayes factor [41], which summarizes the evidence for association from the expected spectrum of PDVs, based on gene size and nucleotide content, to that observed in the sample. Comparing this Bayes factor, we used log10(BF) to account for the vast range of BF values, for the 102 inferred ASD genes identified in Satterstrom et al. [13] to the burden harbored by ASD subjects carrying PDVs in those genes, we do not find a significant relationship with log10(BF) (b =  − 0.015; P = 0.214), although the relationship is negative, as expected.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Distribution of risk scores for different subsets of subjects. Unaffected, ASD PDV carriers with copy number variants (ASD-CNV), loss of function variants (ASD-PTV), and missense variants (ASD-MIS) and ASD non-carriers (ASD-NO-PDV)

Discussion

Here, we asked how rare and common risk variation jointly affect liability for ASD. We analyzed two samples characterized for both types of variation. Based on genotypes of common variation, we computed a small set of risk scores, each of which is likely to describe a portion of the genetic risk of ASD attributable to common variation. We also computed a weighted average of these scores, WGRS, which tended to perform better than any single score at differentiating ASD and unaffected subjects (Fig. 3) and at differentiating ASD subjects who carried rare PDVs likely to affect risk—PDV carriers (Fig. 5)—from ASD subject who were not known to carry such variants (non-carriers). By contrasting patterns of the expected and observed burden of common risk variation in PDV carriers and non-carriers (Fig. 4), we conclude that the preponderance of evidence suggests that rare and common risk variation combine additively in their effects on ASD liability. This agrees with conclusions from other researchers [7, 10, 42].

It is worthwhile emphasizing, however, that the evidence presented here is far from conclusive and it differs from that of other studies. Consider the study by Weiner and colleagues [7], which includes many of the authors of this current manuscript as collaborators. It introduces the pTDT, which uses three key pieces of information to evaluate association: a previously established PRS function; the average of the PRS of mother and father, the mid-parent average; and the deviation of the offspring from the mid-parent average. Using this information, they show that the pTDT is an effective tool for genetically discriminating ASD probands from their unaffected siblings. Moreover, they establish that both carriers and non-carriers, as groups, carry a stochastically greater burden of common risk variants relative to unaffected siblings. Yet, carriers have a pTDT score of 0.17, on average, somewhat but not significantly greater than the average score for non-carriers, 0.12 (their Additional file 1: Table S13). Analyzing a broader set of developmental disabilities, Niemi and colleagues [42] report similar findings to those of Weiner and colleagues [7], specifically carriers have genetic scores indistinguishable from non-carriers and both carry greater burden of risk variation. Both studies use the pTDT approach, and both conclude that rare and common variations combine additively to affect risk.

In contrast to those results [7, 42], in our study the average burden for carriers falls between that for unaffected and non-carrier affected individuals and this we view as evidence for additive effects. The conclusions of our studies are not completely at odds, although they do not fit perfectly together either. That the burden of common risk variants is greater in carriers and non-carriers in all three studies, relative to expectation, is consistent with common variation contributing to liability. What remains unresolved is how it combines with rare variants if they also have a large impact on liability. If PDVs found in individuals with ASD or severe developmental disability were close to completely penetrant—thus having a large impact on liability—then little or no contribution from common variation would be necessary for a diagnosis. Under this model, effects of PDVs are sufficient to cause developmental disability [42] or ASD [7]; common variation would induce variation about the mean liability for affected individuals and perhaps alter presentation of the phenotype. This is observed for quantitative phenotypes of other rare genetic disorders [10]. However, in this scenario the expected liability arising from common variation in carriers should be near zero, as opposed to the significant positive estimates found by Weiner [7] and Niemi [42]. What explains the excess of common variation found in the carriers from their studies? One reasonable possibility is that stochastic variation plays a complicating role. If some PDVs were of sufficient impact on liability to cause the ASD or other developmental phenotype, whereas others were not, and if the impact of this latter group on liability combined additively with common variation, then the fraction of each type of PDV would determine where the mean liability of carrier subjects fell on the continuum between unaffected and non-carrier subjects. Such a model would induce greater variability in the average score for carriers, perhaps sufficiently to make the average burden estimated from carriers and non-carriers indistinguishable.

With larger samples than presented here, more compelling evidence could be drawn from an evaluation of carriers of PDVs in genes with very different recurrence rates in ASD individuals. For example, certain genes, such as CHD8 [38,39,40], are often found to carry PDVs in ASD individuals. Other genes show significant association, yet far less recurrence. In future studies and with a much larger sample, we should be able to order ASD risk genes accurately in terms of the relative risk of ASD generated by PDVs in these genes and evaluate how common variant risk changes along this ranking. If the two sources of risk work additively, they should show a strong negative relationship.

Why do we need to evaluate the nature of the relationship between common and rare variations so thoroughly? Suppose, for example, that some of the ASC’s 102 ASD genes do not truly affect risk and half of the assumed PDV carriers have PDVs in these genes. Under this unlikely but not impossible scenario, these subjects would, in expectation, carry the mean WGRS observed in non-carriers (Fig. 4). To achieve the mean WGRS observed for the entire population of assumed PDV carriers, which consists of an equal mixture of true carriers of risk PDVs and non-carriers, the mean for the subpopulation of true carriers would, in expectation, fall at the mean for unaffected individuals (Fig. 4). Under this scenario, joint effects of rare and common risk variants are irrelevant, a rare PDV would always be sufficient to cause ASD. Such scenarios can only be completely ruled out by using alternative ways of evaluating whether rare and common risk variation combine additively in their effects on ASD liability.

If common variant risk burden of PDV carriers is substantial, as the results here suggest, they have implications for genetic counseling regarding recurrence risk of ASD. Currently, genetic counseling for recurrence risk is binary, depending on whether or not a rare PDV in an ASD gene is found in the proband’s genome. If such a PDV is found, then the PDV is typically assumed “causal” for the proband’s ASD and recurrence probability for ASD is its prevalence. When this assumption is a good approximation, and it will be for many PDV carriers (9], counseling is also a good approximation. In some families, however, the PDV carrier has ASD in large part because of the polygenic burden carried by the parents and in this instance the current advice for recurrence risk is inaccurate. To give a concrete example, when we examined loss-of-function carriers in the SSC, we estimated that over 40% of these individuals would still have ASD even without the loss-of-function PDV [9], and this would be predicted to be even more of an issue with less penetrant variation (e.g., missense variation). Because the present state of knowledge does not allow us to know, a priori, which scenario is relevant, it is important for genetic counselors to consider this uncertainty and whether it should be built into their advice for parents regarding recurrence risk.

Limitations

An ideal study would have even larger sample size than the one we present here. Given the incomplete knowledge of genes and PDVs in ASD, a small fraction of subjects that are categorized as non-carriers could be PDV carriers. In addition, not all DNA from PAGES ASD subjects was characterized for rare sequence variants through exome sequencing and they were assumed to be non-carriers because the vast majority would be. Inherent in the name PDV, we cannot be certain all of these variants actually carry risk of ASD. Indeed, in final review an editor asked for a set of potentially damaging CNVs (pdCNVs) to be re-evaluated for this reason. While taking away these pdCNVs, which removes 28 ASD PDV carriers from our analyses, had little impact on our results (Additional file 1: Table S14), it underscores this uncertainty as a limitation to our analyses. Finally, it is possible that some of the individuals participating in the eMERGE study, who we assumed were unaffected, could be affected (see [43]). These drawbacks limit our ability to go beyond coarse characterization of interplay of rare and common variations.

Conclusion

While rare and common variations confer liability for ASD, how they jointly confer liability is an open question. By analyzing data from 3011 affected subjects and 3011 genetically matched unaffected subjects, we conclude that the burden of common risk variants borne by ASD subjects is stochastically greater than that borne by control subjects and that ASD subjects who carry rare potentially damaging variation conferring risk of ASD have an average burden intermediate between non-carrier ASD and control subjects. The effects of common and rare variants likely combine additively to determine individual-level liability.

Availability of data and material

All results from analyses are presented in the manuscript and Additional file 1.

Abbreviations

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

ASD-NO-PDV:

Non-carrier ASD case

ASD-PDV:

PDV carrier ASD case

CLS:

Cluster-specific

CNV:

Copy number variant

eMERGE:

Electronic MEdical Records and Genomics Network

G-BLUP:

Genomic-Best Linear Unbiased Prediction

GCTA:

Genome-wide complex trait analysis

GP:

Genomic prediction

GRM:

Genomic relationship matrix

GWAS:

Genome-wide association studies

HRC:

Haplotype Reference Consortium

LD:

Linkage disequilibrium

MIS:

De novo missense variant

PAGES:

Population-Based Autism Genetics and Environment Study

pdCNV:

Potentially damaging CNV

PDV:

Potentially damaging variant

POP:

Population

PRS:

Polygenic risk score

pTDT:

Polygenic transmission disequilibrium test

PTV:

Protein truncating variant

QC:

Quality control

SSC:

Simons Simplex Collection

SCZ:

Schizophrenia

TDT:

Transmission disequilibrium test

WGRS:

Weighted genomic risk score

References

  1. Chaste P, Roeder K, Devlin B. The Yin and Yang of autism genetics: how rare de novo and common variations affect liability. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. 2017;18:167–87.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  2. Anney R, Klei L, Pinto D, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, Baird G, et al. Individual common variants exert weak effects on the risk for autism spectrum disorders. Hum Mol Genet. 2012;21(21):4781–92.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Autism Spectrum Disorders Working Group of The Psychiatric Genomics C. Meta-analysis of GWAS of over 16,000 individuals with autism spectrum disorder highlights a novel locus at 10q24.32 and a significant overlap with schizophrenia. Mol Autism. 2017;8:21.

  4. Klei L, Sanders SJ, Murtha MT, Hus V, Lowe JK, Willsey AJ, et al. Common genetic variants, acting additively, are a major source of risk for autism. Mol Autism. 2012;3(1):9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Chaste P, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Hus V, Murtha MT, Lowe JK, et al. A genome-wide association study of autism using the Simons Simplex Collection: Does reducing phenotypic heterogeneity in autism increase genetic homogeneity? Biol Psychiatry. 2015;77(9):775–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, Mattheisen M, Walters RK, Won H, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(3):431–44.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  7. Weiner DJ, Wigdor EM, Ripke S, Walters RK, Kosmicki JA, Grove J, et al. Polygenic transmission disequilibrium confirms that common and rare variation act additively to create risk for autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet. 2017;49(7):978–85.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  8. Leblond CS, Cliquet F, Carton C, Huguet G, Mathieu A, Kergrohen T, et al. Both rare and common genetic variants contribute to autism in the Faroe Islands. NPJ Genom Med. 2019;4:1.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  9. Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Bodea CA, Goldberg AP, Lee AB, et al. Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat Genet. 2014;46(8):881–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  10. Oetjens MT, Kelly MA, Sturm AC, Martin CL, Ledbetter DH. Quantifying the polygenic contribution to variable expressivity in eleven rare genetic disorders. Nat Commun. 2019;10(1):4897.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  11. Fahed AC, Wang M, Homburger JR, Patel AP, Bick AG, Neben CL, et al. Polygenic background modifies penetrance of monogenic variants for tier 1 genomic conditions. Nat Commun. 2020;11(1):3635.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  12. Vuckovic D, Bao EL, Akbari P, Lareau CA, Mousas A, Jiang T, et al. The polygenic and monogenic basis of blood traits and diseases. Cell. 2020;182(5):1214-31.e11.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Satterstrom FK, Kosmicki JA, Wang J, Breen MS, De Rubeis S, An JY, et al. Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cell. 2020;180(3):568-84.e23.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  14. Sanders SJ, He X, Willsey AJ, Ercan-Sencicek AG, Samocha KE, Cicek AE, et al. Insights into autism spectrum disorder genomic architecture and biology from 71 Risk Loci. Neuron. 2015;87(6):1215–33.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Iossifov I, O’Roak BJ, Sanders SJ, Ronemus M, Krumm N, Levy D, et al. The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Nature. 2014;515(7526):216–21.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  16. De Rubeis S, He X, Goldberg AP, Poultney CS, Samocha K, Cicek AE, et al. Synaptic, transcriptional and chromatin genes disrupted in autism. Nature. 2014;515(7526):209–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Fischbach GD, Lord C. The Simons Simplex Collection: a resource for identification of autism genetic risk factors. Neuron. 2010;68(2):192–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  18. Gottesman O, Kuivaniemi H, Tromp G, Faucett WA, Li R, Manolio TA, et al. The Electronic Medical Records and Genomics (eMERGE) network: past, present, and future. Genet Med. 2013;15(10):761–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  19. Purcell SM, Wray NR, Stone JL, Visscher PM, O’Donovan MC, Sullivan PF, et al. Common polygenic variation contributes to risk of schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Nature. 2009;460(7256):748–52.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  20. Schizophrenia Working Group of the Psychiatric Genomics C. Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci. Nature. 2014;511(7510):421–7.

  21. Stahl EA, Breen G, Forstner AJ, McQuillin A, Ripke S, Trubetskoy V, et al. Genome-wide association study identifies 30 loci associated with bipolar disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(5):793–803.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  22. Henderson CR. Best linear unbiased estimation and prediction under a selection model. Biometrics. 1975;31(2):423–47.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  23. de los Campos G, Gianola D, Allison DB. Predicting genetic predisposition in humans: the promise of whole-genome markers. Nat Rev Genet. 2010;11(12):880–6.

  24. Martin AR, Daly MJ, Robinson EB, Hyman SE, Neale BM. Predicting polygenic risk of psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. 2019;86(2):97–109.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Das S, Forer L, Schonherr S, Sidore C, Locke AE, Kwong A, et al. Next-generation genotype imputation service and methods. Nat Genet. 2016;48(10):1284–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  26. McCarthy S, Das S, Kretzschmar W, Delaneau O, Wood AR, Teumer A, et al. A reference panel of 64,976 haplotypes for genotype imputation. Nat Genet. 2016;48(10):1279–83.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  27. Chang CC, Chow CC, Tellier LC, Vattikuti S, Purcell SM, Lee JJ. Second-generation PLINK: rising to the challenge of larger and richer datasets. Gigascience. 2015;4:7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Lee AB, Luca D, Klei L, Devlin B, Roeder K. Discovering genetic ancestry using spectral graph theory. Genet Epidemiol. 2010;34(1):51–9.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Yang J, Lee SH, Goddard ME, Visscher PM. GCTA: a tool for genome-wide complex trait analysis. Am J Hum Genet. 2011;88(1):76–82.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  30. Yang J, Benyamin B, McEvoy BP, Gordon S, Henders AK, Nyholt DR, et al. Common SNPs explain a large proportion of the heritability for human height. Nat Genet. 2010;42(7):565–9.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  31. Luca D, Ringquist S, Klei L, Lee AB, Gieger C, Wichmann HE, et al. On the use of general control samples for genome-wide association studies: genetic matching highlights causal variants. Am J Hum Genet. 2008;82(2):453–63.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Bodea CA, Neale BM, Ripke S, Daly MJ, Devlin B, Roeder K. A Method to exploit the structure of genetic ancestry space to enhance case–control studies. Am J Hum Genet. 2016;98(5):857–68.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  33. Yip BHK, Bai D, Mahjani B, Klei L, Pawitan Y, Hultman CM, et al. Heritable variation, with little or no maternal effect, accounts for recurrence risk to autism spectrum disorder in Sweden. Biol Psychiatry. 2018;83(7):589–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Samocha KE, Kosmicki, J.A., Karczewski, K.J., O'Donnell-Luria, A.H., Pierce-Hoffman, E., MacAuthor, D.G., Neale, B.M., Daly, M.J. Regional missense constraint improves variant deleteriousness prediction. bioRxiv. 2017. https://doi.org/10.1101/148353.

  35. An JY, Lin K, Zhu L, Werling DM, Dong S, Brand H, et al. Genome-wide de novo risk score implicates promoter variation in autism spectrum disorder. Science. 2018;362:6420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Mahjani B, De Rubeis S, Gustavsson Mahjani C, Mulhern M, Xu X, Klei L, et al. Prevalence and phenotypic impact of rare potentially damaging variants in autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2021. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00465-3 (in press)

  37. Devlin B, Roeder K. Genomic control for association studies. Biometrics. 1999;55(4):997–1004.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Neale BM, Kou Y, Liu L, Ma’ayan A, Samocha KE, Sabo A, et al. Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2012;485(7397):242–5.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  39. Sanders SJ, Murtha MT, Gupta AR, Murdoch JD, Raubeson MJ, Willsey AJ, et al. De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature. 2012;485(7397):237–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Bernier R, Golzio C, Xiong B, Stessman HA, Coe BP, Penn O, et al. Disruptive CHD8 mutations define a subtype of autism early in development. Cell. 2014;158(2):263–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  41. He X, Sanders SJ, Liu L, De Rubeis S, Lim ET, Sutcliffe JS, et al. Integrated model of de novo and inherited genetic variants yields greater power to identify risk genes. PLoS Genet. 2013;9(8):e1003671.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  42. Niemi MEK, Martin HC, Rice DL, Gallone G, Gordon S, Kelemen M, et al. Common genetic variants contribute to risk of rare severe neurodevelopmental disorders. Nature. 2018;562(7726):268–71.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  43. Raspa M, Paquin RS, Brown DS, Andrews S, Edwards A, Moultrie R, et al. Preferences for accessing electronic health records for research purposes: views of parents who have a child with a known or suspected genetic condition. Value Health. 2020;23(12):1639–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank everyone who contributed to this study, both the research subjects and the investigators of the following studies: Simons Simplex Collection: We would like to thank the SSC principal investigators (A. L. Beaudet, R. Bernier, J. Constantino, E. H. Cook, Jr, E. Fombonne, D. Geschwind, D. E. Grice, A. Klin, D. H. Ledbetter, C. Lord, C. L. Martin, D. M. Martin, R. Maxim, J. Miles, O. Ousley, B. Peterson, J. Piggot, C. Saulnier, M. W. State, W. Stone, J. S. Sutcliffe, C. A. Walsh and E. Wijsman) and the coordinators and staff at the SSC clinical sites; the SFARI staff, in particular N. Volfovsky; D. B. Goldstein for contributing to the experimental design; and the Rutgers University Cell and DNA repository for accessing biomaterials. Electronic MEdical Records and Genomics Network: Group Health Cooperative/University of Washington—Funding support for Alzheimer's Disease Patient Registry (ADPR) and Adult Changes in Thought (ACT) study was provided by a U01 from the National Institute on Aging (Eric B. Larson, PI, U01AG006781). A gift from the 3M Corporation was used to expand the ACT cohort. DNA aliquots sufficient for GWAS from ADPR Probable AD cases, who had been enrolled in Genetic Differences in Alzheimer's Cases and Controls (Walter Kukull, PI, R01 AG007584) and obtained under that grant, were made available to eMERGE without charge. Funding support for genotyping, which was performed at Johns Hopkins University, was provided by the NIH (U01HG004438). Genome-wide association analyses were supported through a Cooperative Agreement from the National Human Genome Research Institute, U01HG004610 (Eric B. Larson, PI). Mayo Clinic—Samples and associated genotype and phenotype data used in this study were provided by the Mayo Clinic. Funding support for the Mayo Clinic was provided through a cooperative agreement with the National Human Genome Research Institute (NHGRI), Grant #: UOIHG004599; and by grant HL75794 from the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (NHLBI). Funding support for genotyping, which was performed at The Broad Institute, was provided by the NIH (U01HG004424). Marshfield Clinic Research Foundation—Funding support for the Personalized Medicine Research Project (PMRP) was provided through a cooperative agreement (U01HG004608) with the National Human Genome Research Institute (NHGRI), with additional funding from the National Institute for General Medical Sciences (NIGMS) The samples used for PMRP analyses were obtained with funding from Marshfield Clinic, Health Resources Service Administration Office of Rural Health Policy grant number D1A RH00025, and Wisconsin Department of Commerce Technology Development Fund contract number TDF FYO10718. Funding support for genotyping, which was performed at Johns Hopkins University, was provided by the NIH (U01HG004438). Northwestern University—Samples and data used in this study were provided by the NUgene Project (www.nugene.org). Funding support for the NUgene Project was provided by the Northwestern University’s Center for Genetic Medicine, Northwestern University, and Northwestern Memorial Hospital. Assistance with phenotype harmonization was provided by the eMERGE Coordinating Center (Grant number U01HG04603). This study was funded through the NIH, NHGRI eMERGE Network (U01HG004609). Funding support for genotyping, which was performed at The Broad Institute, was provided by the NIH (U01HG004424). Vanderbilt University—Funding support for the Vanderbilt Genome-Electronic Records (VGER) project was provided through a cooperative agreement (U01HG004603) with the National Human Genome Research Institute (NHGRI) with additional funding from the National Institute of General Medical Sciences (NIGMS). The dataset and samples used for the VGER analyses were obtained from Vanderbilt University Medical Center's BioVU, which is supported by institutional funding and by the Vanderbilt CTSA grant UL1RR024975 from NCRR/NIH. Funding support for genotyping, which was performed at The Broad Institute, was provided by the NIH (U01HG004424). Assistance with phenotype harmonization and genotype data cleaning was provided by the eMERGE Administrative Coordinating Center (U01HG004603) and the National Center for Biotechnology Information (NCBI). The datasets used for the analyses described in this manuscript were obtained from dbGaP at http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/gap through dbGaP accession number phs000360.v3.p1.

Funding

This work was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grants R37MH057881 (to B.D. and K.R.), R01MH097849 (to J.D.B.), U01MH111661 (to J.D.B.), and U01MH111658 (to B.D. and K.R.); a Simons Foundation grant (SF575547) to K.R., B.D., and Haiyuan Yu; and the Seaver Foundation (to J.D.B, S.D.R., B. M., and S.S.).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

L.K., K.P., S.DR., N.M., A.R., S.S., C.M.H., J.D.B., K.R., and B.D. conceived the study; A.C.S.G. and G.K. recruited subjects, with guidance from A.R., S.S., C.M.H., and J.D.B.; L.K., L.M., B.M., K.P., Y.L., and X.X. performed computational analyses with input from J.D.B., K.R., and B.D; and L.K., L.M., B.M., S.DR., S.C., A.R., S.S., C.M.H., J.D.B., K.R., and B.D. wrote the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bernie Devlin.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Not applicable.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1.

Supplementary information for joint effects of rare and common variation.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Klei, L., McClain, L.L., Mahjani, B. et al. How rare and common risk variation jointly affect liability for autism spectrum disorder. Molecular Autism 12, 66 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00466-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-021-00466-2

Keywords