Skip to main content

Empathic disequilibrium in two different measures of empathy predicts autism traits in neurotypical population

Abstract

Background

Features of autism spectrum conditions (ASC) are normally distributed within the population, giving rise to the notion of the autism spectrum. One of the hallmark features of ASC is difficulties in social communication, which relies heavily on our ability to empathize with others. Empathy comprises of both cognitive (CE) and emotional (EE) components that, together, allow us to understand another’s emotions and be affected by them appropriately, while maintaining a self-other distinction. Although CE and EE depend on distinct neural and developmental trajectories, it was suggested that the two empathic capacities can influence, balance, and regulate each other. Previous findings regarding the role of emotional and cognitive empathy in ASC have been mixed. Therefore, our study aimed to investigate whether the intra-personal empathy imbalance between the cognitive and emotional components, a measure we termed empathic disequilibrium (ED), can be associated with autism traits at the neurotypical range.

Methods

Participants were 671 young-adults at the neurotypical range who self-reported their empathy, assessed using two highly validated questionnaires—the Interpersonal Reactivity Index and the Empathy Quotient, autism traits using the Autism-Spectrum Quotient, and the related traits, alexithymia, and systemizing.

Results

Controlling for the total empathy score, greater ED was found to be positively correlated with autism traits. Specifically, autism traits were found to be elevated in groups of individuals with relatively higher EE than CE, underscoring their imbalance.

Conclusions

Our study offers a novel perspective on the understanding of the social difficulties associated with autism tendencies in the general population and has potentially important clinical implications for understanding of ASC. We also propose a novel characterization of autism tendencies based on the imbalance between EE and CE, which we term ED, as opposed to examining EE and CE separately.

Background

Autism spectrum conditions (ASC) are a behaviorally diagnosed set of neurodevelopmental conditions characterized by difficulties in social interaction and communication, accompanied by repetitive and restrictive behaviors with onset during early development [1]. Individuals with ASC display considerable variability in many features including cognitive, emotional, biological, and developmental aspects [2,3,4]. This phenotypic heterogeneity suggests that ASC is indeed heterogeneous, representing an aggregation of multiple conditions [5]. Studies consistently show that milder, yet similar, characteristics of the defining features of ASC are continuously distributed across the general population, representing both familial and genetic liability to ASC [6, 7]. Accordingly, individuals with a clinical diagnosis of ASC are viewed as the extreme end of a continuum of autism traits, which at some point cross the line into the clinical phenotype [8]. Moreover, similar to ASC phenotypes, this subclinical population also presents significant heterogeneity [9, 10].

Therefore, studying the autism spectrum in the general population is informative to clarifying the multiple characteristics in individuals who are genetically and epigenetically predisposed to ASC, thus elucidating the nature of ASC and its variability [11]. The aim of the current study was to focus on the origins of the social difficulties related to ASC by examining whether disequilibrium in emotional versus cognitive empathy contributes to autism tendencies in the general population.

Although tremendous variability is evidenced in ASC phenotypes, difficulties in social communication are indisputably fundamental to its definition [12]. Human ability for social communication relies heavily on our ability to empathize with others [13]. Consistently, many studies suggest that deficits in empathy are prevalent in autism [14,15,16,17]. Therefore, studying the relationship between empathy and autism tendencies can inform our understanding of ASC.

Emotional and cognitive empathy

Empathy, defined as the ability to understand another’s emotions and be affected by them appropriately, while maintaining a self-other distinction, includes both emotional and cognitive components [18]. Emotional empathy (EE) is the ability to respond to another’s mental states with an appropriate emotion, while cognitive empathy (CE) is the ability to recognize what another person is feeling [14]. CE is closely related to the affective aspects of Theory of Mind, which is defined as the ability to make inferences regarding other’s emotions [19], whereas EE includes an emotional response to other’s internal states [20]. Oftentimes, the measurement of EE focuses on the emotional response to another’s distress, which can take two forms. One form is that of feeling of compassion or concern towards the other, termed sympathy or empathic concern. Another form is the tendency to experience distress or discomfort in response to distress in others, termed personal distress [21].

CE and EE have different developmental and neural trajectories. For instance, CE continuously increases throughout early childhood and adolescence, while EE appears very early on and remains relatively stable throughout the lifespan [22,23,24,25]. Furthermore, neuro-imaging studies suggest that CE tasks activate brain regions such as ventromedial prefrontal cortex and the temporo-parietal junction, while EE tasks relate to the amygdala and cingulate cortex structures [19, 26,27,28]. The distinction between the two empathy components has been supported by genetic studies as well [29,30,31,32].

Although CE and EE depend on distinct neural networks, overlapping neural response between the two empathic capacities was found in brain regions such as the anterior insula [33, 34]. Accordingly, it was suggested that EE and CE can influence, balance, and regulate each other while simultaneously retaining a significant degree of independence and that both functions are jointly required in complex social situations [35,36,37,38]. This has been shown in a meta-analysis study of empathy for pain, in which brain activation was compared between two types of empathy tasks [28]. In a picture-based task, participants were presented with visual depictions of someone in a painful situation, while in the cue-based task, participants were presented with only a cue/hint that someone else is receiving painful stimulation. In addition to a common activation in the two paradigms, CE and EE brain regions were co-activated in the cue-based task. This suggests that CE-related brain regions interact with EE-related brain regions in complex and unclear or ambiguous social situations, in which additional processing is needed to jointly engage EE and CE with the feelings of the other. As complex and relatively ambiguous social situations are constantly encountered in everyday life, these studies suggest that maintaining a balance between EE and CE is crucial for an adaptive and appropriate social response, leading to effective social communication.

Relatedly, deficits in CE and EE were found to be dissociable in a wide array of psychiatric conditions including schizophrenia [39], anti-social personality disorder [16], obsessive-compulsive disorder [40], and bipolar disorder [41].

Emotional and cognitive empathy in ASC

The dissociation between CE and EE was also suggested to be a hallmark of individuals with ASC [14]. The empathy imbalance hypothesis (EIH) proposes that individuals with ASC show impaired CE, while maintaining high EE functioning and that this imbalance contributes to some autism symptoms [36]. In line with this hypothesis, many empirical findings based on self-report [42, 43] and neurobehavioral [15, 44, 45] measures of empathy show impaired CE in individuals with ASC, while EE is either exaggerated or remains intact.

While these studies converge on a specific pattern of empathy imbalance in individuals with ASC, others show mixed and inconsistent results [17, 46, 47]. For instance, one study [46] found that young children with autism displayed EE less frequently compared to children at the neurotypical range. This reduced responsiveness could not be explained by a failure to look at the experimenters’ emotional displays. In another study [4], the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET), a task measuring CE by asking participants to recognize others’ mental states from the eyes region of the face [48, 49], was used to classify individuals with ASC into five separate subgroups, two of which did not differ in RMET score from individuals at the neurotypical range. The researchers suggested the notion that there are ASC subgroups which do not show lower CE.

One explanation for these findings might be that CE and EE by themselves are not sensitive enough to characterize ASC, and there is a need to consider the intra-personal variability between EE and CE, while previous studies examined EE and CE separately. For example, we suggest that individuals showing average CE, are also characterized by even higher EE and it is the imbalance between the traits which is at the root of the social difficulties displayed in autism. Thus, we suggest there is a need to jointly examine the balance between CE and EE, and not, as carried out in most previous investigations, only each of these components individually.

To our knowledge, currently, only one empirical study quantified the intra-personal imbalance between CE and EE directly [50]. In this study, the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) [21], a commonly used self-report measure of empathy, was used to derive a new measure investigating the intra-personal empathy of each individual, termed “relative empathic ability” (REA). The researchers showed differences in functional connectivity between individuals with EE-dominance, showing stronger connectivity among social-emotional regions, and individuals with CE-dominance, showing stronger social-cognitive processing and interoceptive network connectivity. REA was also associated with some symptoms of psychopathology, that could not be otherwise explained by CE and EE separately. Although this study found no association between REA and autism traits in participants at the neurotypical range, their analysis was based on only 18 participants, a very small sample size likely lacking power to detect the sought after effect, suggesting that further investigations, with larger sample size, are needed.

The current study

Our study aimed to investigate whether intra-personal empathy imbalance can be associated with autism traits in the general population. Based on Cox et al.’s [50] findings regarding the neurobiological and behavioral implication of REA, and the empathic imbalance hypothesis [36], we preferred to use the term “empathic disequilibrium” (ED) to define this imbalance, thus capturing its possible clinical implications. For our purpose, we created an ED measure, derived separately from two highly validated self-report questionnaires of empathy, the IRI [21] and the Empathy Quotient (EQ) [14]. We used both measures separately to make sure that findings are not measure-dependent. We hypothesized that ED will be positively correlated with autism traits in the general population. We also explored the differences in autism traits between the two ED groups of individuals (EE-dominant and CE-dominant). To better define each group, we further investigated other autism-related traits that were previously proposed to characterize ASC and its subgroups including alexithymia, a subclinical condition characterized by difficulties in identifying and describing one’s own emotional state [51] and systemizing, the drive to analyze, or construct systems [52, 53].

Methods

Participants

A total of 671 college students (56% females; mean age 24.5 ± 2.5) were recruited by word of mouth and advertisements on Israeli institution campuses. All of the participants filled out the EQ, and 629 of those completed the full battery of questionnaires, as listed below. One participant was removed from the IRI analyses due to unusually low EE and CE scores on the questionnaire (< − 4 standard deviations), leaving 628 participants in the IRI analyses. This number of participants should provide sufficient power to detect effects of small size (f2 > 0.02). Participants were of Jewish descent, with no self-reported history of psychiatric disorders, chronic illness, or drug taking. All participants were paid volunteers. This sample was previously analyzed and described by Uzefovsky et al. [32]. As expected of the general population, all measurements including Autism-Spectrum Quotient, Toronto Alexithymia Scale, Systemizing Quotient, EQ, and IRI in this sample (see Table 1 for means) fell within the average range previously reported in neurotypical population [21, 53,54,55] with only five participants who scored above, or equal to, the Autism-Spectrum Quotient clinical cut-off score of 32 [56].

Table 1 Descriptive statistics—measures of autism-related traits and empathy

Measures

Participants came to the lab, where informed consent was obtained. Participants were then given access to an online, password-protected platform. There, participants filled out a demographic questionnaire and completed a battery of questionnaires measuring empathy and autism-related traits.

Empathy measures

Empathy was measured using two different highly validated empathy questionnaires. These measures were analyzed according to validated emotional and cognitive empathy subscales.

Interpersonal reactivity index (IRI) [21]

The questionnaire consists of 28 items on a 5-point scale, which can be divided into four validated subscales, each made up of seven items. Two of the subscales measure CE (perspective taking (PT) and fantasizing (F)) and two subscales measure EE (empathic concern (EC) and personal distress (PD)).

Empathy quotient (EQ) [14]

The questionnaire consists of 60 items (40 empathy items and 20 filler items) on a 4-point scale. On each empathy item, a person can score 2, 1, or 0. The EQ consists of three validated factors: “cognitive empathy” (11 items), “emotional reactivity” (11 items), and “social skills” (6 items) [57, 58]. In the current study, we focused on the first two subscales and did not include “social skills” as it does not directly relate to EE or CE. The “emotional reactivity” scale was used as a measure of EE. An example of an item is “I tend to get emotionally involved with a friend’s problems”. The authors define emotional reactivity as the tendency to have an emotional reaction in response to others’ mental states, which is similar to the definition of EE. Although “emotional reactivity” does not include aspects of personal distress, as does the EE scale of the IRI, it does capture the sharing in other’s emotional experience. Thus, we treated this factor as a measure of EE, similarly to other numerous studies (e.g., [59,60,61,62]).

Autism-related measures

Autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ) [56]

This questionnaire consists of 50-items measuring autism traits in the general population. All items in this measure are scored on a four-point rating scale, with higher total score indicating higher autism traits. The AQ can also be divided according to five domains: “social skill,” “attention switching,” “attention to detail,” “communication,” and “imagination.”

Toronto alexithymia scale (TAS-20) [63]

This is a 20-item measure designed to assess alexithymia, which is defined as a difficulty in describing and identifying one’s emotional state. A higher score on this questionnaire indicates higher alexithymia. TAS-20 has three subscales: “difficulty describing feelings,” “difficulty identifying feelings,” and “externally-oriented thinking” (made up of 5, 7, and 8 items, respectively).

Systemizing quotient (SQ) [53]

This is a 60-item (40 systemizing items and 20 filler items) questionnaire with a 0–2 rating scale aimed to assess systemizing disposition, which is the drive to analyze or construct systems.

Descriptive statistics of each measure used in this study are shown in Table 1. Raw Pearson’s correlations matrix of all the measures is shown in Table 2.

Table 2 Raw correlation matrix

Empathic disequilibrium calculation

Using the CE and EE scores derived from each empathy questionnaire (IRI and EQ) separately, we created two indices to represent and quantify ED.

  1. (1)

    ED was calculated as (standardized CE score − standardized EE score), thus representing individuals’ relative differences (in standard deviation) between CE and EE. We used standardized scores as it provides a meaningful scoring system (a score of 1 represents one standard deviation difference between EE and CE) and allows to avoid biases that might affect the comparability between the two traits (e.g., subjects may be prone to expectancy bias in only one scale). In this measure, a positive score indicates CE-dominance, while a negative score indicates EE-dominance.

  2. (2)

    ED-magnitude is the ED score in absolute value, indicating the level of disparity between emotional and cognitive empathy. Thus, a high ED-magnitude score indicates an imbalance between cognitive and emotional empathy, while a low ED-magnitude score confers a balance between the two traits.

Statistical analysis

Two types of analyses were preformed, for each of the empathy measures, to answer our main questions. (1) To examine whether ED-magnitude uniquely contributes to the prediction of autism traits, we conducted multiple regression analyses with ED-magnitude derived from IRI and EQ separately. In each analysis, we predicted AQ score using empathy score (from IRI or EQ) and its derivative ED-magnitude score. We controlled for sex and age in all analyses. To make sure ED-magnitude uniquely contributes to the prediction of autism traits, similar analyses were conducted with EE and CE instead of the general empathy score.

(2) To examine the differences between CE-dominant versus EE-dominant ED scores, we grouped the participants according to their ED scores to CE-dominant ( 1 SD), EE-dominant (≤ −1 SD) and balanced empathy (between − 1 and 1 SD) groups. Characteristics of participants in each group are shown in Table 3. A plot of the correlation between CE and EE scores, (r = 0.43, p = 7 × 10−32 in EQ; r = 0.365, p = 3 × 10−21 in IRI) grouped by ED dominance is shown in Fig. 1.

Table 3 Empathy group characteristics
Fig. 1
figure 1

Correlation between CE and EE. Distribution of CE and EE scores derived from IRI (left panel) and EQ (right panel) per participant. Empathic (dis)equilibrium groups are represented using color, showing EE-dominance in grey, balanced empathy in orange, and CE-dominance in blue. CE, cognitive empathy; EE, emotional empathy; IRI, Interpersonal Reactivity Index; EQ, Empathy Quotient

Differences between the three groups (EE-dominance/CE-dominance/balanced empathy) in autism-related traits, measured by AQ, TAS-20, and SQ, were separately examined using a one-way ANOVA. Sex was used as a covariate. We assigned a strict Bonferroni-corrected p value of (0.05/3 tests = 0.017) to account for multiple testing. The analyses of the AQ and TAS-20 subscales can be found in Additional File 1. We further investigated the reliability of ED, by analyzing ED scores calculated using two additional methods. The first such ED was calculated based on a mean of the relevant subscales originating from the IRI and EQ. The second, based on participants whose group assignment to the ED groups was identical based on both measures. Details regarding these analyses and their outcomes appear in Additional File 1. All statistical analyses mentioned above were carried out using R v3.6.1 using the stats [64] and emmeans [65] packages.

Results

IRI-derived ED analyses

Applying multiple regression analysis with age and sex as covariates, ED-magnitude was found to be positively correlated with AQ score (β = 0.124, p = 0.004), while the total IRI score failed to predict AQ (β = 0.05, p = 0.24). In this analysis, AQ score was neither explained by age (β = 0.04, p = 0.32) nor sex (β = − 0.01, p = 0.8). Findings were similar when controlling for EE and CE (instead of a total empathy score); a positive correlation was observed between ED-magnitude and AQ score (β = 0.14, p = 0.001). Neither age (β = 0.04, p = 0.32) nor sex (β = − 0.01, p = 0.81) predicted AQ score. AQ score was negatively correlated with CE (β = − 0.21, p = 7 × 10−6), but no correlation was found between AQ score and EE (β = 0.06, p = 0.16).

We next grouped participants based on their ED scores. As described in Table 3, participants in each group did not differ in age (F = 0.86, p = 0.43, ƞp2 = 0.003) and total IRI score (F = 0.29, p = 0.75, ƞp2 < 0.001). Relative to the whole sample, sex differed in the CE-dominant ED group (χ2(1, N = 112) = 8.95, p = 0.003) so that this group had more males than females (58%).

Results of the one-way ANOVA examinations of autism traits between the three groups are described in Table 4 (AQ and TAS-20 subscales results are described in Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Table 2 in Additional File 1, respectively). These analyses revealed significant differences between the three groups in AQ, TAS-20, and SQ scores.

Table 4 IRI-derived ED analysis of empathy groups

To better understand the origins of these associations, further analyses were conducted contrasting each of the ED groups (EE-dominance and CE-dominance, separately) with the balanced empathy group. To account for multiple testing, we used a strict Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.01 (0.05/3 significant measures × 2 contrasts). The results of these analyses are summarized in Fig. 2. AQ and TAS-20 subscales analyses are displayed in Supplementary Figure 1, Additional File 1. Analyzing the total AQ score revealed higher autism traits in individuals with EE-dominance (t(619) = 4.86, p = 1 × 10−6, ƞp2 = 0.037). No difference was found between the CE-dominance and balanced empathy groups (t(619) = − 0.57, p = 0.57, ƞp2 = 0.0005). Individuals with balanced empathy scored lower on TAS-20, as compared to the EE-dominance group (t(619) = 3.39, p = 7 × 10−4, ƞp2 = 0.02) and scored higher compared to the CE-dominance group (t(619) = − 4.01, p = 7 × 10−5, ƞp2 = 0.025). Our analyses also revealed significantly higher SQ score in the CE-dominance group as compared to the balanced empathy group (t(619) = 4.72, p = 2 × 10−5, ƞp2 = 0.034). Nominally significant lower SQ scores were found in the EE-dominance group compared to the balanced empathy group (t(619) = − 2.02, p = 0.04, ƞp2 = 0.006).

Fig. 2
figure 2

IRI-derived ED further analysis. Results of further analyses showing differences between high IRI-derived ED groups (EE-dominance and CE-dominance) and the balanced empathy group in AQ, TAS-20, and SQ. IRI, Interpersonal Reactivity Index; AQ, Autism Quotient; TAS-20, Toronto Alexithymia Scale; SQ, Systemizing Quotient. *p < 0.01, **p < 0.001, ***p < 0.0001

EQ-derived ED analyses

The same analyses were conducted calculating ED using the EQ. Similarly to the IRI analysis, multiple regression, with age and sex as covariates, showed ED-magnitude to be positively correlated with AQ score and uniquely contributed to its prediction (β = 0.12, p = 0.0045). In this analysis, the total EQ score also predicted AQ (β = − 0.3, p = 5 × 10−12). AQ score was neither explained by age (β = 0.06, p = 0.12) nor sex (β = 0.08, p = 0.075). Findings were similar when controlling for EE and CE (instead of a total empathy score), a positive correlation between ED-magnitude and AQ score (β = 0.13, p = 0.002). Neither age (β = 0.06, p = 0.14) nor sex (β = 0.04, p = 0.36) predicted AQ score. AQ score was also negatively correlated with CE (β = − 0.2, p = 2 × 10−5), but no correlation was found between AQ score and EE (β = − 0.07, p = 0.144).

After grouping the participants based on their EQ-derived ED score (see Table 3), no age differences were found between the three groups (F = 0.55, p = 0.57, ƞp2 = 0.002). However, the total EQ score significantly differed between the groups (F = 3.141, p = 0.044, ƞp2 = 0.01), so that the EQ score in the CE-dominant ED group was slightly lower than the EQ score in the balanced empathy group (t(668) = − 2.45, p = 0.015, ƞp2 = 0.01). No difference in EQ score was found between the EE-dominance and balanced empathy groups (t(668) = − 1.02, p = 0.31, ƞp2 = 0.001). Chi-square analyses revealed sex differences in both EE-dominance and CE-dominance groups (χ2(1, N = 109) = 8.33, p = 0.004, and χ2(1, N = 113) = 21.7, p < 0.0001, respectively) so that the EE-dominance group included more females than males (70%), and the CE-dominance ED group included more males than females (65%).

Results of the one-way ANOVA examination of autism-related traits between the three groups are described in Table 5 (AQ and TAS-20 subscales results are described in Supplementary Table 1 and Supplementary Table 2, respectively). These analyses revealed significant differences between the three groups in AQ and SQ scores. No differences in TAS-20 were found.

Table 5 EQ-derived ED analysis of empathy groups

Applying the same method used in the IRI analyses for the EQ-derived ED groups, further analyses were conducted contrasting the ED groups (CE-dominance and EE-dominance separately) with the balanced empathy group. Multiple testing was accounted for using a strict Bonferroni-corrected p < 0.0125 (0.05/2 significant measures × 2 contrasts). The results of this analysis are displayed in Fig. 3. Similarly to the IRI analyses, the contrasts examining the AQ scores between EE-dominance and balanced empathy groups revealed a significant difference, indicating higher autism traits in the EE-dominance group (t(623) = 3.72, p = 2 × 10−4, ƞp2 = 0.02). No such differences were found between the balanced empathy and the CE-dominance group (t(619) = 0.71, p = 0.48, ƞp2 = 0.001). Relative to individuals in the balanced empathy group, lower SQ score was found in the EE-dominance group (t(623) = − 3.1, p = 0.002, ƞp2 = 0.015). SQ did not differ between the balanced empathy group and the CE-dominance group (t(619) = 1.93, p = 0.05, ƞp2 = 0.006).

Fig. 3
figure 3

EQ-derived ED further analysis. Differences between ED (EE-dominance and CE-dominance) and balanced empathy groups derived from the EQ measure in AQ and SQ scores. AQ, Autism Quotient; SQ, Systemizing Quotient. *p < 0.0125, **p < 0.00125, ***p < 0.000125

Analyses of the relationship between ED and the subscales of AQ and TAS-20 in addition to analyses examining ED as calculated based on the mean of EQ and IRI, as well as based on compatible groupings on both measures, can be found in Additional File 1.

Discussion

In the current study, we investigated whether the intra-personal empathy imbalance between the cognitive and emotional components, a measure we termed ED, is associated with autism traits at the neurotypical range of the autism spectrum. Our analyses revealed that the size of ED is positively correlated with autism traits. We further showed that autism traits are elevated specifically in a group of individuals with relatively higher EE than CE. These results were robust and consistent across two different highly validated measures of empathy. ED groups also associated with other autism-related traits showing differences in systemizing and alexithymia.

The positive correlation between ED-magnitude and autism traits supports the novel notion that measuring the imbalance between the cognitive and emotional components of empathy can provide an informative and meaningful predictor of ASC-related traits in the general population. We suggest that the previous attempt by Cox et al. [50] to link autism traits to empathy imbalance in the general population fell short due to lack of power (small sample size of N = 18).

We next showed that ED association with autism traits could not be merely explained by participants’ general ability to empathize, as the later was controlled for in our regression analysis. This finding is also reflected in the ED group analyses, revealing no general empathy differences between EE-dominance and the balanced empathy groups in both measures of empathy. These results suggest that ED and the interplay between CE and EE are independent predictors of autism and autism-related traits.

Further investigating ED, we specifically discovered higher autism traits in individuals showing EE-dominant ED. This finding provides empirical evidence supporting the general notion of EIH which argues that the cognitive and behavioral characteristics of individuals with ASC are an adaptive response to over-arousal caused by the imbalance between CE and EE [36]. This suggests a novel hypothesis, to be tested in future research, that certain ASC group(s) can be characterized better through the concept of ED, rather than deficits in EE or CE separately. We further suggest that our results might address the conundrum of the mixed or heterogeneous findings in previous studies. For instance, it is possible that groups of individuals with autism who show unimpaired CE (e.g. [4]) also show higher than typical EE, and this could shed light on their experienced social difficulties. Therefore, individuals with autism who display typical levels of CE might be better understood through the lens of ED.

Further examining ED in autism might also shed light on the phenotypical heterogeneity of ASC that is apparent at the neural level [66]. For instance, fMRI studies reveal conflicting evidence of both increased and decreased resting-state functional connectivity [67, 68]. In their paper, Cox et al. [50] showed that dominance of EE was associated with stronger resting-state functional connectivity between socio-emotional regions of the brain such as the ventral anterior insula, orbital-frontal cortex, amygdala, and perigenual anterior cingulate. Connectivity between these regions was found to be altered in individuals with ASC [69, 70]. Furthermore, hyper-connectivity between the amygdala and the ventral anterior insula, regions which are co-activated by emotional stimuli [71], was previously associated with anxiety [72]. As might be hypothesized based on the EIH, individuals with EE-dominance might display elevated autism traits alongside over-arousal and anxiety elicited by sensitivity to external stimuli. Taken together, it is reasonable to hypothesize that this hyper-connectivity in socio-emotional networks might reflect a neural propensity defining a subgroup of individuals specifically affected by EE-dominance, and future studies could ascertain this hypothesis.

To better understand the features of the EE-dominant ED group, we examined associations with autism-related traits in the general population. We found that the EE-dominant group is characterized by intact or even lower systemizing propensity. This finding is surprising as this group was also related to higher AQ, while higher systemizing is characteristic of ASC [53, 73]. This seeming disparity might be explained by the specific association previously found between systemizing and the non-social aspects of autism (i.e., repetitive behavior and stereotyped interests). For instance, a recent genome-wide association study (GWAS) found genetic correlation between systemizing and the non-social, but not with the social aspects of ASC (i.e. social interaction and communication) [74]. Together with the lower systemizing found in the EE-dominant ED group, this might hint that EE-dominant ED is particularly related to the social difficulties of ASC, but not the non-social aspects of ASC.

The link between EE-dominant ED and social deficits prevalent in autism gains further support as individuals in this group showed heightened alexithymia in the IRI-derived ED. Alexithymia reflects difficulties in identifying and describing own emotions [51, 75], and although alexithymia is not a diagnostic feature of ASC it is widely prevalent in individuals with autism. It is also common in relatives of individuals with ASC and was suggested to be a feature of the broader autism phenotype [76]. As alexithymia and ASC share many overlapping features, alexithymia was suggested to play a complex role in ASC and to contribute to the social and emotional deficits displayed in autism [51, 77,78,79]. Nevertheless, the current finding should be taken cautiously as no differences in TAS-20 were found in the EQ-derived ED group analysis. Importantly, the empathy constructs tapped by the IRI and EQ are not identical, such that the IRI-derived EE subscale contains both empathic concern and personal distress, while the EQ-derived EE subscale focuses on one’s own emotional response to others’ emotions, and these differences might explain the inconsistent result. More specifically, ED as measured by the IRI, highlights a response to negative states/emotions, encompassing both empathic concern and personal distress as measures of EE. In contrast, the EQ-derived EE does not tap into personal distress caused by the other’s negative emotions. Rather it focuses on more neutral valenced states/emotions and more generally on the emotional reactions to others’ emotional states. Indeed, the replication of the main findings across these two different measures (and using different methods of calculating ED based on the two measures, see Additional File 1) and conceptualizations of EE strengthens the generalizability of our findings. At the same time, this distinction might explain our findings as alexithymia components, measured by the TAS-20, were previously associated with negative but not positive emotions [80, 81].

Finally, investigating the specific pattern of sex differentiation between ED groups in our data is also of interest. We found a significantly higher female-to-male ratio in the EE-dominant group, higher male-to-female ratio in the CE-dominant group, while no sex differences were found in the balanced empathy group. This finding is surprising as the EE-dominant group was associated with higher autism traits, which are far more prevalent in males than in females in clinical and in the general population [73, 82, 83]. This seeming discrepancy may in fact serve as a hint that ED might be particularly related to female-typical manifestations of autism, although this needs to be interpreted cautiously as females are over-represented in our sample and the opposite is true for autism as a diagnosis [84]. Furthermore, as females diagnosed with ASC is a relatively under-studied population that is not well characterized [84,85,86], we suggest that examination of sex differences in future research of ED in autism will be of value.

Interestingly, sex differentiation in empathy imbalance was previously suggested in the EIH, suggesting the theory that natural selection acted separately on EE and CE, shaping the interaction between the two capacities in a sex-dependent way [38]. According to this theory, men will be more susceptible than females to empathy imbalance and would facilitate male competitive, aggressive, and violent behavior. Conversely, reduced imbalance between the two capacities will characterize females and would facilitate behaviors such as child-rearing, tendencies selected for during human evolution [87]. The current findings show that the level of ED does not differentiate between the sexes. Rather, both males and females show ED but females tend to show EE-dominance whereas males tend to show CE-dominance. These results beg further investigation into the possible social and biological origins of this difference.

Limitations

It is important to stress that all traits measured in this study were self-report questionnaires reflecting the participants’ perception of their own functioning and ability. Although all measures used are highly validated and were previously found to be correlated with other self-report and behavioral measures [14, 53, 56, 63, 88], self-report measures are prone to be over/underestimation of the subjects’ actual abilities. Thus, future research should address this limitation, using behavioral measures of empathy to further validate the ED concept.

Another limitation in the current study is the attempt to investigate autism traits solely within the neurotypical range, and not including participants diagnosed with ASC. On the one hand, we did not examine whether the population was truly neurotypical using diagnostic measures, although only five participants in the sample scored above the AQ clinical cut off [56]. On the other hand, examining autism traits in a sample of individuals seemingly at the neurotypical range limits the interpretability of the findings to the typical range of the autism spectrum. Future studies should examine these effects in a sample of individuals with ASC. Indeed, the effect sizes reported here are small to moderate. If a greater imbalance between EE and CE is associated with a diagnosis, stronger effect sizes can be expected.

The dissociation between EE and CE characterizes a wide range of psychiatric conditions [16, 39,40,41]; therefore, future studies should explore the role of ED in other clinical conditions. In this regard, it should also be mentioned that although we suggest ED is related to clinical conditions, the CE-dominant ED group was not characterized by higher autism traits nor alexithymia. However, it did show relatively higher systemizing scores and this finding should be further explored in other clinical traits associated with both higher systemizing and empathy, such as obsessive-compulsive personality disorder [89] and positive symptoms in schizophrenia [90]. Investigating other clinical traits with known empathy deficits such as anti-social personality disorder [16] should also be of interest.

Conclusions

In this study, we showed that the intra-personal imbalance between the emotional and cognitive aspects of empathy offers a very novel way of understanding and measuring empathy as a construct and we predict is likely to be of prognostic value to autism traits in the general population towards early diagnosis and prevention. Our results are robust and were consistent using two different highly validated measures of empathy.

Our study adds to the understanding of the entire autism spectrum and sheds light on its mechanisms and variability in the general population. Based on empirical findings supporting empathic disequilibrium, the findings imply the possibility of novel subgroup classification of ASC based on the imbalance between EE and CE, rather by CE or EE separately, thus bridging the gap on current issues in the literature of ASC.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets generated and/or analyzed during the current study are not publicly available because consent from participants did not cover data sharing. The data sets are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ASC:

Autism spectrum conditions

EE:

Emotional empathy

CE:

Cognitive empathy

EIH:

Empathy imbalance hypothesis

RMET:

Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test

REA:

Relative empathic ability

ED:

Empathic disequilibrium

IRI:

Interpersonal Reactivity Index

PT:

Perspective taking

F:

Fantasizing

EC:

Empathic concern

PD:

Personal distress

EQ:

Empathy Quotient

AQ:

Autism-Spectrum Quotient

TAS-20:

Toronto Alexithymia Scale

SQ:

Systemizing Quotient

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-5®): American Psychiatric Pub; 2013.

  2. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Duku E, Bennett TA, Bryson S, Fombonne E, et al. Developmental trajectories of symptom severity and adaptive functioning in an inception cohort of preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(3):276–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Willsey AJ, State MW. Autism spectrum disorders: from genes to neurobiology. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2015;30:92–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Lombardo MV, Lai M-C, Auyeung B, Holt RJ, Allison C, Smith P, et al. Unsupervised data-driven stratification of mentalizing heterogeneity in autism. Sci Rep. 2016;6:35333.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Geschwind DH, Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2007;17(1):103–11.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Hurst RM, Mitchell JT, Kimbrel NA, Kwapil TK, Nelson-Gray RO. Examination of the reliability and factor structure of the autism Spectrum quotient (AQ) in a non-clinical sample. Personal Individ Differ. 2007;43(7):1938–49.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Verweij CJH, Boomsma DI. Heritability of autistic traits in the general population. JAMA Pediatr. 2007;161(4):372–7.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Lundström S, Chang Z, Råstam M, Gillberg C, Larsson H, Anckarsäter H, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autisticlike traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69(1):46–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Bora E, Aydın A, Saraç T, Kadak MT, Köse S. Heterogeneity of subclinical autistic traits among parents of children with autism spectrum disorder: identifying the broader autism phenotype with a data-driven method. Autism Res. 2017;10(2):321–6.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Wheelwright S, Auyeung B, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Defining the broader, medium and narrow autism phenotype among parents using the autism Spectrum quotient (AQ). Molecular Autism. 2010;1(1):10.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Sucksmith E, Roth I, Hoekstra RA. Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: re-examining the broader autism phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychol Rev. 2011;21(4):360–89.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Frye RE. Social skills deficits in autism spectrum disorder: potential biological origins and progress in developing therapeutic agents. CNS Drugs. 2018;32(8):713–34.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  13. Decety J, Bartal IB-A, Uzefovsky F, Knafo-Noam A. Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2016;371(1686):20150077.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences. J Autism Dev Disord. 2004;34(2):163–75.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Dziobek I, Rogers K, Fleck S, Bahnemann M, Heekeren HR, Wolf OT, et al. Dissociation of cognitive and emotional empathy in adults with Asperger syndrome using the multifaceted empathy test (MET). J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(3):464–73.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Jones AP, Happé FGE, Gilbert F, Burnett S, Viding E. Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2010;51(11):1188–97.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Mazza M, Pino MC, Mariano M, Tempesta D, Ferrara M, De Berardis D, et al. Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. 2014;8:791.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Decety J, Jackson PL. The functional architecture of human empathy. Behav Cogn Neurosci Rev. 2004;3(2):71–100.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D. Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain. 2009;132(3):617–27.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Zaki J, Ochsner KN. The neuroscience of empathy: progress, pitfalls and promise. Nat Neurosci. 2012;15(5):675–80.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Davis MH. A multidimensional approach to individual difference in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology. 1980;10:85.

  22. Hutman T, Dapretto M. The emergence of empathy during infancy. Cognitie, Creier, Comportament. 2009;13(4):367–90.

  23. Hoffman ML. Development of prosocial motivation: empathy and guilt. In: (Ed.) NE, editor. The development of prosocial behavior. New York: Academic Press; 1982. p. 281-313.

  24. Uzefovsky F, Knafo-Noam A. Empathy development throughout the life span. Social Cognition: Routledge; 2016. p. 89-115.

  25. Knafo A, Zahn-Waxler C, Van Hulle C, Robinson JL, Rhee SH. The developmental origins of a disposition toward empathy: genetic and environmental contributions. Emotion. 2008;8(6):737–52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Völlm BA, Taylor ANW, Richardson P, Corcoran R, Stirling J, McKie S, et al. Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage. 2006;29(1):90–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Schnell K, Bluschke S, Konradt B, Walter H. Functional relations of empathy and mentalizing: an fMRI study on the neural basis of cognitive empathy. NeuroImage. 2011;54(2):1743–54.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Lamm C, Decety J, Singer T. Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. NeuroImage. 2011;54(3):2492–502.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Uzefovsky F, Shalev I, Israel S, Edelman S, Raz Y, Mankuta D, et al. Oxytocin receptor and vasopressin receptor 1a genes are respectively associated with emotional and cognitive empathy. Horm Behav. 2015;67:60–5.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Huetter FK, Bachmann HS, Reinders A, Siffert D, Stelmach P, Knop D, et al. Association of a common oxytocin receptor gene polymorphism with self-reported ‘empathic concern’ in a large population of healthy volunteers. PLoS One. 2016;11(7):e0160059.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Smith KE, Porges EC, Norman GJ, Connelly JJ, Decety J. Oxytocin receptor gene variation predicts empathic concern and autonomic arousal while perceiving harm to others. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9(1):1–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Uzefovsky F, Shalev I, Israel S, Edelman S, Raz Y, Perach-Barzilay N, et al. The dopamine D4 receptor gene shows a gender-sensitive association with cognitive empathy: evidence from two independent samples. Emotion. 2014;14(4):712–21.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Lamm C, Singer T. The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Struct Funct. 2010;214(5):579–91.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G. Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2011;35(3):903–11.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Preckel K, Kanske P, Singer T. On the interaction of social affect and cognition: empathy, compassion and theory of mind. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2018;19:1–6.

    Google Scholar 

  36. Smith A. The empathy imbalance hypothesis of autism: a theoretical approach to cognitive and emotional empathy in autistic development. Psychol Rec. 2009;59(3):489–510.

    Google Scholar 

  37. Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. Sci World J. 2006;6:1146–63.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Smith A. Cognitive empathy and emotional empathy in human behavior and evolution. Psychol Rec. 2006;56(1):3–21.

    Google Scholar 

  39. Atoui M, El Jamil F, El Khoury J, Doumit M, Syriani N, Khani M, et al. The relationship between clinical insight and cognitive and affective empathy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn. 2018;12:56–65.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Fontenelle LF, Soares ID, Miele F, Borges MC, Prazeres AM, Rangé BP, et al. Empathy and symptoms dimensions of patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Psychiatr Res. 2009;43(4):455–63.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Cusi A, MacQueen GM, McKinnon MC. Altered self-report of empathic responding in patients with bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res. 2010;178(2):354–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Rueda P, Fernández-Berrocal P, Baron-Cohen S. Dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in youth with Asperger syndrome. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2015;12(1):85–98.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A. Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(4):709–15.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Magnée MJCM, De Gelder B, Van Engeland H, Kemner C. Facial electromyographic responses to emotional information from faces and voices in individuals with pervasive developmental disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2007;48(11):1122–30.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Gleichgerrcht E, Torralva T, Rattazzi A, Marenco V, Roca M, Manes F. Selective impairment of cognitive empathy for moral judgment in adults with high functioning autism. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2012;8(7):780–8.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  46. Scambler DJ, Hepburn S, Rutherford MD, Wehner EA, Rogers SJ. Emotional responsivity in children with autism, children with other developmental disabilities, and children with typical development. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007;37(3):553–63.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. aan het Rot M, Hogenelst K. The influence of affective empathy and autism spectrum traits on empathic accuracy. PLoS One. 2014;9(6):e98436.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test revised version: a study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. 2001;42(2):241–51.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  49. Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, Allison C, Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, et al. The “Reading the mind in the eyes” test: complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PLoS One. 2015;10(8):e0136521.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  50. Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C. The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain's intrinsic functional dynamics. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2011;7(6):727–37.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  51. Bird G, Cook R. Mixed emotions: the contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl Psychiatry. 2013;3(7):e285–e.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  52. Baron-Cohen S. Autism and the Empathizing–Systemizing (ES) theory. Developmental social cognitive neuroscience: Psychology Press; 2016. p. 139-52.

  53. Baron-Cohen S, Richler J, Bisarya D, Gurunathan N, Wheelwright S. The systemizing quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism, and normal sex differences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond Ser B Biol Sci. 2003;358(1430):361–74.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Ruzich E, Allison C, Chakrabarti B, Smith P, Musto H, Ring H, et al. PLoS One. 2015;10(10):e0141229–e.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  55. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. The 20-item Toronto alexithymia scale: IV. Reliability and factorial validity in different languages and cultures. J Psychosom Res. 2003;55(3):277–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31(1):5–17.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Lawrence EJ, Shaw P, Baker D, Baron-Cohen S, David AS. Measuring empathy: reliability and validity of the empathy quotient. Psychol Med. 2004;34(5):911–20.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Muncer SJ, Ling J. Psychometric analysis of the empathy quotient (EQ) scale. Personal Individ Differ. 2006;40(6):1111–9.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Greenberg DM, Baron-Cohen S, Rosenberg N, Fonagy P, Rentfrow PJ. Elevated empathy in adults following childhood trauma. PLoS One. 2018;13(10):e0203886–e.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Montgomery CB, Allison C, Lai M-C, Cassidy S, Langdon PE, Baron-Cohen S. Do adults with high functioning autism or Asperger syndrome differ in empathy and emotion recognition? J Autism Dev Disord. 2016;46(6):1931–40.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  61. Groen Y, den Heijer AE, Fuermaier ABM, Althaus M, Tucha O. Reduced emotional empathy in adults with subclinical ADHD: evidence from the empathy and systemizing quotient. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord. 2018;10(2):141–50.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Maurage P, Grynberg D, Noël X, Joassin F, Philippot P, Hanak C, et al. Dissociation between affective and cognitive empathy in alcoholism: a specific deficit for the emotional dimension. Alcohol Clin Exp Res. 2011;35(9):1662–8.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale—I. item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res. 1994;38(1):23–32.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. Available online at https://www.R-project.org/; 2014.

  65. Lenth R, Singmann H, Love J, Buerkner P, Herve M. emmeans: estimated marginal means, aka least-squares means. R package version 1.3. 5. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package = emmeans; 2018.

  66. Zabihi M, Oldehinkel M, Wolfers T, Frouin V, Goyard D, Loth E, et al. Dissecting the heterogeneous cortical anatomy of autism spectrum disorder using normative models. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. 2019;4(6):567–78.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Hull JV, Dokovna LB, Jacokes ZJ, Torgerson CM, Irimia A, Van Horn JD. Resting-state functional connectivity in autism spectrum disorders: a review. Frontiers in Psychiatry. 2017;7(205).

  68. Tang S, Sun N, Floris DL, Zhang X, Di Martino A, Yeo BTT. Reconciling dimensional and categorical models of autism heterogeneity: a brain connectomics and behavioral study. bioRxiv. 2019:692772.

  69. Delmonte S, Gallagher L, O'Hanlon E, Mc Grath J, Balsters J. Functional and structural connectivity of frontostriatal circuitry in Autism Spectrum Disorder. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2013;7(430).

  70. Ebisch SJH, Gallese V, Willems RM, Mantini D, Groen WB, Romani GL, et al. Altered intrinsic functional connectivity of anterior and posterior insula regions in high-functioning participants with autism spectrum disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. 2011;32(7):1013–28.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Viinikainen M, Jääskeläinen IP, Alexandrov Y, Balk MH, Autti T, Sams M. Nonlinear relationship between emotional valence and brain activity: evidence of separate negative and positive valence dimensions. Hum Brain Mapp. 2010;31(7):1030–40.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Baur V, Hänggi J, Langer N, Jäncke L. Resting-state functional and structural connectivity within an insula–amygdala route specifically index state and trait anxiety. Biol Psychiatry. 2013;73(1):85–92.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Greenberg DM, Warrier V, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S. Testing the empathizing–systemizing theory of sex differences and the extreme male brain theory of autism in half a million people. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2018;115(48):12152–7.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Warrier V, Toro R, Won H, Leblond CS, Cliquet F, Delorme R, et al. Social and non-social autism symptoms and trait domains are genetically dissociable. Commun Biol. 2019;2(1):328.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  75. Griffin C, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B. Alexithymia in children with and without autism spectrum disorders. Autism Res. 2016;9(7):773–80.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Szatmari P, Georgiades S, Duku E, Zwaigenbaum L, Goldberg J, Bennett T. Alexithymia in parents of children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(10):1859–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  77. Gerber AH, Girard JM, Scott SB, Lerner MD. Alexithymia – not autism – is associated with frequency of social interactions in adults. Behav Res Ther. 2019;123:103477.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Poquérusse J, Pastore L, Dellantonio S, Esposito G. Alexithymia and autism spectrum disorder: a complex relationship. Front Psychol. 2018;9:1196.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  79. Cook R, Brewer R, Shah P, Bird G. Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci. 2013;24(5):723–32.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  80. Suslow T, Donges U-S. Alexithymia components are differentially related to explicit negative affect but not associated with explicit positive affect or implicit affectivity. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1758.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  81. Zhang L, Ye R, Yu F, Cao Z, Zhu C, Cai Z, et al. How does emotional context modulate response inhibition in alexithymia: electrophysiological evidence from an ERP study. PLoS One. 2012;7(12):e51110–e.

    CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  82. Ruzich E, Allison C, Smith P, Watson P, Auyeung B, Ring H, et al. Measuring autistic traits in the general population: a systematic review of the autism-Spectrum quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Molecular Autism. 2015;6(1):2.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  83. Lai M-C, Lombardo MV, Auyeung B, Chakrabarti B, Baron-Cohen S. Sex/gender differences and autism: setting the scene for future research. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2015;54(1):11–24.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  84. Loomes R, Hull L, Mandy WPL. What is the male-to-female ratio in autism spectrum disorder? A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2017;56(6):466–74.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Kreiser NL, White SW. ASD in females: are we overstating the gender difference in diagnosis? Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2014;17(1):67–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Hull L, Petrides KV, Mandy W. The female autism phenotype and camouflaging: a narrative review. Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 2020.

  87. Taylor SE. Tend and befriend: biobehavioral bases of affiliation under stress. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2006;15(6):273–7.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  88. Montalan B, Lelard T, Godefroy O, Mouras H, et al. Front Psychol. 2012;3:389.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  89. Cain NM, Ansell EB, Simpson HB, Pinto A. Interpersonal functioning in obsessive–compulsive personality disorder. J Pers Assess. 2015;97(1):90–9.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Russell-Smith SN, Bayliss DM, Maybery MT, Tomkinson RL. Are the autism and positive schizotypy spectra diametrically opposed in empathizing and systemizing? J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(3):695–706.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

Support was received from the National Institute for Psychobiology in Israel—founded by the Charles E. Smith family (233-17-18), and by the Arianne de Rothschild fellowship. IS was supported by the Interfaculty Brain Science school at Ben Gurion University in Israel and by the Paedagogica Foundation.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

IS designed the current study, analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. FU designed the current study and provided critical input on data analyses and the writing of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Florina Uzefovsky.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures contributing to this work complied with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Declaration of Helsinki of 1975, as revised in 2008.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1.

Results and discussion of supplementary analyses. Supplementary Table 1. AQ subscales analyses. Supplementary Table 2. TAS-20 subscales analyses. Supplementary Table 3. Combined empathy score analyses. Supplementary Table 4. Concurrent group analyses. Supplementary Figure 1. Further analyses of the AQ and TAS-20 subscales based on IRI-derived ED groups. Supplementary Figure 2. Further analyses of the AQ subscales based on EQ-derived ED groups.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Shalev, I., Uzefovsky, F. Empathic disequilibrium in two different measures of empathy predicts autism traits in neurotypical population. Molecular Autism 11, 59 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00362-1

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-020-00362-1

Keywords