Skip to main content

No relationship between early postnatal testosterone concentrations and autistic traits in 18 to 30-month-old children

Abstract

Background

Some previous research has suggested that testosterone prenatally contributes to gender differences in autistic traits, but little is known about the role of testosterone during early postnatal development (mini-puberty). Two prior studies found no sex difference in testosterone postnatally in saliva samples and detected little to no relationship between testosterone postnatally and autistic traits in toddlers. These findings may reflect late measurements of testosterone at 3 to 4 months of age, after the peak of mini-puberty at 1 to 3 months of age. The present study examined the relationship between testosterone at 1 to 3 months of age and autistic traits at 18 to 30 months of age.

Findings

Testosterone was measured in saliva samples collected from children at 1 to 3 months of age. When the children (40 boys, 47 girls) reached 18 to 30 months of age, parents completed the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT). Boys had higher concentrations of testosterone postnatally and higher Q-CHAT scores than girls. However, testosterone did not correlate with Q-CHAT scores in boys, girls, or the entire sample.

Conclusions

The current results suggest that testosterone during the early postnatal period does not contribute to later autistic traits. Given our relatively small samples and therefore limited power, however, further research could usefully examine if testosterone in saliva samples collected during the peak of mini-puberty in larger groups predicts autistic traits or other traits that show gender differences.

Findings

Background

One of the most consistent findings in autism spectrum conditions (ASCs) research is that males are more likely than females to be diagnosed with the conditions [1]. In addition, in the general population, males on average score higher on measures of autistic traits than females [25].

Studying the causal mechanisms involved in this gender bias may provide insights into the aetiology of ASCs. It has been proposed that ASCs reflect an ‘extreme male brain (EMB)’ and that androgens during early development are a key neurobiological factor underlying development of an EMB [6, 7]. Some prior studies have found that testosterone prenatally is significantly related to subsequent autistic traits [811]. A recent study compared 128 males with an ASC diagnosis to 217 matched controls without an ASC diagnosis and found no significant difference in testosterone that had been measured in their amniotic fluid [12]. That same study, however, found that the ASC group scored significantly higher than the controls on a latent steroidogenic factor that was derived from measures of five hormones (progesterone, 17α-hydroxy-progesterone, androstenedione, testosterone, and cortisol) assayed in amniotic fluid [12].

Little is known about whether androgen exposure during early infancy relates to later autistic traits. There is a transient surge of sex steroids, called ‘mini-puberty’ , in male infants, with testosterone concentrations peaking at about 1 to 3 months postnatal, before declining to low childhood values by around 6 months of age [13, 14]. Testosterone during mini-puberty has been shown to influence development of the male genitalia and reproductive function [15] and has been associated with postnatal penile and testicular growth in healthy infants [16]. Since brain plasticity remains high throughout the early postnatal period [17], it is possible that mini-puberty contributes to neurobehavioral sexual differentiation, as well.

Two studies have examined the influences of testosterone during mini-puberty on autistic traits. These studies related testosterone assayed from saliva samples collected at 3 to 4 months of age to parent-reported autistic traits during toddlerhood. The first study found no correlation between testosterone and scores on the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT) in boys (n = 15), girls (n = 20), or the entire sample at age 18 to 35 months [8]. The second study reported a significant positive correlation between testosterone and scores on a subscale of the Brief Infant–Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment (BITSEA) in 47 boys and 37 girls combined at age 18 to 24 months but did not report any within-sex correlation [18]. Notably, neither study found a sex difference in testosterone concentrations, perhaps because saliva samples were collected after the peak of mini-puberty. One of the studies also found no gender difference in autistic traits [18]. The lack of differences between boys and girls limits the studies’ implications for the neurobehavioral effects of mini-puberty on autistic traits.

The present study explored the relationship between salivary testosterone during the peak of mini-puberty, at 1 to 3 months of age, and parent-reported autistic traits, as assessed by the Q-CHAT, at 18 to 30 months of age. Saliva sampling was employed since it affords a non-invasive approach. The Q-CHAT was used because prior studies have found gender differences in Q-CHAT scores [2, 8].

Methods

Participants and design

Participants were recruited from a larger longitudinal study investigating the influences of testosterone during mini-puberty on child development. The pattern of the postnatal surge appears to differ for full term (37 or more weeks of gestation) and preterm infants [16], so only full term infants were recruited. Parents of 87 (47 girls) healthy, full term infants were recruited in Cambridgeshire, England, and saliva samples were taken from these infants for testosterone assays when they were 1 to 3 months old. Parents (85 mothers, 2 fathers) completed an online questionnaire assessing their children’s autistic traits when the children were 18 to 30 months old (mean age = 22.39 months; SD = 3.44 months; range = 18.00–30.32 months). In the current sample, most of the children are of Caucasian descent (87.4 %); the rest are of mixed (11.5 %) or Indian/Pakistani/Bangladeshi descent (1.1 %). Parents provided informed consents for their own and their children’s participation. The study was approved by the Psychology Research Ethics Committee at Cambridge University.

Outcome variable

Autistic traits were measured by the Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers (Q-CHAT) [2], a 25-item parent-report questionnaire answered using a 5-point Likert scale. It has been validated in an unselected sample of children at the age of 18 to 24 months and in children with ASCs at the age of 19 to 63 months [2]. It has good test-retest reliability after 1 month (r = .82) [2]. Individual item scores are summed to obtain a Q-CHAT total score, ranging from 0 to 100. Higher scores indicate more autistic traits. Internal consistency (Cronbach’s α) based on all items of the Q-CHAT was .67 in the current sample of 87 children.

Predictor variable

Saliva was collected from the infant using a small-sized inert polymer swab at the age of 4 to 14 weeks (mean age = 7.82 weeks; SD = 1.96 weeks; range = 4.14–14.43 weeks). Testosterone exhibits a diurnal rhythm, with the highest concentrations in the morning and the lowest concentrations around midnight [19, 20], so saliva samples were collected between 8:30 am and 12 noon. There was no correlation between testosterone and sample collection time within this 3.5-h window in a subsample of 60 children for whom sampling time was recorded (r = .03). Samples were stored at −25 °C before being sent to Salimetrics (Cambridgeshire, England) for testosterone assays. Testosterone concentrations were measured in duplicate using enzyme immunoassays (assay sensitivity <1 pg/ml, intra-assay coefficient of variation = 5 %, inter-assay coefficient of variation = 10 %). One male infant produced insufficient saliva for hormonal assay. Therefore, data on testosterone concentrations were available for 39 boys and 47 girls.

Control variables

Birth weight, child’s age at saliva sampling and at Q-CHAT assessment, maternal and paternal age and education level, and number of siblings were included as control variables. Maternal and paternal education was rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (primary education only) to 5 (postgraduate degree).

Results

There were no outliers and all variables had acceptable skewness. Boys and girls differed significantly in testosterone concentrations and in Q-CHAT scores but not in any other variable (see Table 1 for descriptive, independent sample t test and Cohen’s d statistics). There were no significant bivariate correlations between Q-CHAT scores and testosterone or any control variable in boys, girls, or the entire sample (see Table 2 for Pearson’s correlation statistics and Fig. 1 for a scatter plot showing the relationship between Q-CHAT scores and testosterone within each sex).

Table 1 Descriptive and inferential statistics for differences between boys and girls
Table 2 Correlations of Q-CHAT scores to predictor and control variables
Fig. 1
figure 1

Scatter plot showing the relationship between Q-CHAT scores and testosterone at mini-puberty within each sex

Since the Q-CHAT was originally designed for children at the age of 18 to 24 months [2], the above analyses were repeated excluding children older than 24 months (12 boys, 12 girls). Highly comparable results were produced. There were no significant correlations between testosterone and Q-CHAT scores in boys (r = .09), girls (r = .03), or the entire sample (r = .13) for children ages 18 to 24 months.

Conclusions

The present study investigated whether salivary testosterone measured at 1 to 3 months of age relates to autistic traits assessed by the Q-CHAT at 18 to 30 months of age. Consistent with prior studies [2, 8], the present study found that boys scored higher on the Q-CHAT than girls. Whereas prior studies reported no sex difference in salivary testosterone in infants at age 3 to 4 months [8, 18], the present study found that boys had higher concentrations of salivary testosterone than girls at age 1 to 3 months, during the peak of the early postnatal surge (mini-puberty). Despite detecting differences between boys and girls in both testosterone and autistic traits, the present study found no correlation between testosterone and autistic traits in boys, girls, or the entire sample. Although the current results cannot be directly compared with prior findings, due to the differences in the timing of testosterone measures, research findings thus far appear to suggest that testosterone during mini-puberty has little to no effects on autistic traits [8, 18].

In addition to augmenting evidence on testosterone and autistic traits, our findings highlight the potential of saliva sampling as an approach for studying the effects of early androgen exposure on neurobehavioural sexual differentiation. The present study detected a sex difference in salivary testosterone, which resembles prior findings showing sex differences in testosterone in blood [13, 14] and urine [21] samples collected during the peak of the postnatal surge. Because saliva can be obtained non-invasively and conveniently, its use may be particularly well-suited for further research investigating the effects of mini-puberty. Whilst mini-puberty may not contribute to autistic traits, it has been found that urinary testosterone and penile growth during mini-puberty predict gender-typed play behaviour in early childhood [21, 22]. Further research may relate salivary testosterone to other aspects of development that differ by gender.

The current results do not address the influences of testosterone prenatally. There is some evidence suggesting that testosterone exposure during foetal development may relate to later autistic traits [812]. By contrast, prior studies found no relationship between autistic traits and perinatal or postnatal androgen exposure, as measured in umbilical cord blood at birth or saliva after the peak of mini-puberty [8, 18, 23, 24]. Moreover, the present study found no relationship between testosterone at the peak of mini-puberty and autistic traits. Thus, research evidence thus far appears to suggest that early postnatal testosterone does not contribute to autistic traits.

Limitations of the present study include reliance on a small sample, which could reduce statistical power, and the largely Caucasian and highly educated participants (see Table 1), which could reduce generalisability of the findings. Further research with larger and more diverse samples would be of interest.

In conclusion, the present study expands on prior research, suggesting that there is no relationship between testosterone exposure during mini-puberty and autistic traits. This does not preclude effects of mini-puberty on other behaviours, however. Indeed, testosterone during mini-puberty has been found to predict gender-typed play behaviour. Future studies might use early postnatal saliva samples to evaluate the effects of mini-puberty on other traits that show gender differences.

Abbreviations

ASC:

autism spectrum condition

BITSEA:

Brief Infant–Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment

EMB:

extreme male brain

Q-CHAT:

Quantitative Checklist for Autism in Toddlers

References

  1. Halladay AK, Bishop S, Constantino JN, Daniels AM, Koenig K, Palmer K, et al. Sex and gender differences in autism spectrum disorder: summarizing evidence gaps and identifying emerging areas of priority. Mol Autism. 2015;6:36.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Charman T, Richler J, Pasco G, et al. The Q-CHAT (Quantitative CHecklist for Autism in Toddlers): a normally distributed quantitative measure of autistic traits at 18–24 months of age: preliminary report. J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1414–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Allison C. The autism spectrum quotient: children’s version (AQ-Child). J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38:1230–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Baron-Cohen S, Hoekstra RA, Knickmeyer R, Wheelwright S. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ)—adolescent version. J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36:343–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. 2001;31:5–17.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science. 2005;310:819–23.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. 2002;6:248–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Auyeung B, Ahluwalia J, Thomson L, Taylor K, Hackett G, O’Donnell KJ, et al. Prenatal versus postnatal sex steroid hormone effects on autistic traits in children at 18 to 24 months of age. Mol Autism. 2012;3:17.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Auyeung B, Baron‐Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Brit J Psychol. 2009;100:1–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Auyeung B, Taylor K, Hackett G, Baron-Cohen S. Foetal testosterone and autistic traits in 18 to 24-month-old children. Mol Autism. 2010;1:11.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Fane BA, Wheelwright S, Mathews GA, Conway GS, et al. Androgens and autistic traits: a study of individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav. 2006;50:148–53.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Baron-Cohen S, Auyeung B, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Hougaard DM, Abdallah MW, Melgaard L, et al. Elevated fetal steroidogenic activity in autism. Mol Psychiatr. 2015;20:369–76.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Forest MG, Sizonenko PC, Cathiard AM, Bertrand J. Hypophysogonadal function in humans during the first year of life: evidence for testicular activity in early infancy. J Clin Invest. 1974;53:819–28.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Winter JS, Hughes IA, Reyes FI, Faiman C. Pituitary-gonadal relations in infancy: 2. Patterns of serum gonadal steroid concentrations in man from birth to two years of age. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1976;42:679–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Main KM, Schmidt IM, Skakkebaek NE. A possible role for reproductive hormones in newborn boys: progressive hypogonadism with the postnatal testosterone peak. J Clin Endocr Metab. 2005;85:4905–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Kuiri-Hänninen T, Seuri R, Tyrväinen E, Turpeinen U, Hämäläinen E, Stenman UH, et al. Increased activity of the hypothalamic–pituitary–testicular axis in infancy results in increased androgen action in premature boys. J Clin Endocr Metab. 2011;96:98–105.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. de Graaf-Peters VB, Hadders-Algra M. Ontogeny of the human central nervous system: what is happening when? Early Hum Dev. 2006;82:257–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Saenz J, Alexander GM. Postnatal testosterone levels and disorder relevant behavior in the second year of life. Biol Psychol. 2013;94:152–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Ankarberg C, Norjavaara E. Diurnal rhythm of testosterone secretion before and throughout puberty in healthy girls: correlation with 17β-estradiol and dehydroepiandrosterone sulfate. J Clin Endocr Metab. 1999;84:975–84.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Diver MJ, Imtiaz KE, Ahmad AM, Vora JP, Fraser WD. Diurnal rhythms of serum total, free and bioavailable testosterone and of SHBG in middle-aged men compared with those in young men. Clin Endocrinol. 2003;58:710–7.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  21. Lamminmäki A, Hines M, Kuiri-Hänninen T, Kilpeläinen L, Dunkel L, Sankilampi U. Testosterone measured in infancy predicts subsequent sex-typed behavior in boys and in girls. Horm Behav. 2012;61:611–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Pasterski V, Acerini CL, Dunger DB, Ong KK, Hughes IA, Thankamony A, et al. Postnatal penile growth concurrent with mini-puberty predicts later sex-typed play behavior: evidence for neurobehavioral effects of the postnatal androgen surge in typically developing boys. Horm Behav. 2015;69:98–105.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Whitehouse A, Mattes E, Maybery M, Dissanayake C, Sawyer M, Jones R, et al. Perinatal testosterone exposure and autistic-like traits in the general population: a longitudinal pregnancy-cohort study. J Neurodev Disord. 2012;4:25.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Jamnadass E, Keelan J, Hollier L, Hickey M, Maybery M, Whitehouse A. The perinatal androgen to estrogen ratio and autistic-like traits in the general population: a longitudinal pregnancy cohort study. J Neurodev Disord. 2015;7:17.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ES/J500033/1; ES/I901957/1; ES/H016406/1) and the University of Cambridge.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karson T. F. Kung.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

All authors contributed to the development of the study concept, research design, and research questions. KTFK, MC, and WVB collected the data. KTFK performed the data analysis. KTFK drafted the manuscript, and MC, WVB, RMN, and MH provided critical revisions for important intellectual content. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kung, K.T.F., Constantinescu, M., Browne, W.V. et al. No relationship between early postnatal testosterone concentrations and autistic traits in 18 to 30-month-old children. Molecular Autism 7, 15 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0078-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13229-016-0078-8

Keywords